Ihre Suche
Ergebnisse 38 Einträge
-
On duration judgments lasting seconds to minutes, which are thought to be cognitively mediated, women typically perceive time intervals as longer than men do. On a perceptual level in the milliseconds range, few reports indicate higher acuity of temporal processing in men than in women. In this study, sex differences in the perception of temporal order of two acoustic stimuli were identified in neurologically healthy subjects, as well as in brain-injured patients with lesions in either the left or the right hemisphere. Women needed longer interstimulus intervals than men before they were able to indicate the correct temporal order of two clicks. Neurobiological evidence and findings on cognitive strategies are discussed to explain the apparent psychophysical sex differences.
-
Recent research suggests that our sense of time intervals in the range of seconds is directly related to activity in the insular cortex, which contains the primary sensory area for interoception. We therefore investigated whether performance in a duration reproduction task might correlate with individual interoceptive awareness and with measurable changes in autonomic activity during the task. Thirty-one healthy volunteers participated in an interoceptive (heartbeat) perception task and in repeated temporal reproduction trials using intervals of 8, 14, and 20s duration while skin conductance levels and cardiac and respiratory periods were recorded. We observed progressive increases in cardiac periods and decreases in skin conductance level during the encoding and (less reliably) the reproduction of these intervals. Notably, individuals' duration reproduction accuracy correlated positively both with the slope of cardiac slowing during the encoding intervals and with individual heartbeat perception scores. These results support the view that autonomic function and interoceptive awareness underpin our perception of time intervals in the range of seconds.
-
Standard diagnostic procedures for assessing temporal-processing abilities of adult patients with aphasia have so far not been developed. In our study, temporal-order measurements were conducted using two different experimental procedures to identify a suitable measure for clinical studies. Additionally, phoneme-discrimination abilities were tested on the word, as well as on the sentence level, as a relationship between temporal processing and phoneme-discrimination abilities is assumed. Patients with aphasia displayed significantly higher temporal-order thresholds than control subjects. The detection of an association between temporal processing and speech processing, however, depended on the stimuli and the phoneme-discrimination tasks used. Our results also suggest top-down feedback on phonemic processing.
-
PURPOSE: The relationship between auditory temporal-order perception and phoneme discrimination has been discussed for several years, based on findings, showing that patients with cerebral damage in the left hemisphere and aphasia, as well as children with specific language impairments, show deficits in temporal-processing and phoneme discrimination. Over the last years several temporal-order measurement procedures and training batteries have been developed. However, there exists no standard diagnostic tool for adults that could be applied to patients with aphasia. Therefore, our study aimed at identifying a feasible, reliable and efficient measurement procedure to test for auditory-temporal processing in healthy young and elderly adults, which in a further step can be applied to patients with aphasia. METHODS: The tasks varied according to adaptive procedures (staircase vs. maximum-likelihood), stimuli (tones vs. clicks) and stimulation modes (binaural- vs. alternating monaural) respectively. A phoneme-discrimination task was also employed to assess the relationship between temporal and language processing. RESULTS: The results show that auditory temporal-order thresholds are stimulus dependent, age related, and influenced by gender. Furthermore, the cited relationship between temporal-order threshold and phoneme discrimination can only be confirmed for measurements with pairs of tones. CONCLUSION: Our results indicate, that different norms have to be established for different gender and age groups. Furthermore, temporal-order measurements with tones seem to be more suitable for clinical intervention studies than measurements with clicks, as they show higher re-test reliabilities, and only for measurements with tones an association with phoneme-discrimination abilities was found.
-
In experimental studies using flight simulations subjects' duration estimates have shown to be an effective indicator of cognitive task demands. In this study we wanted to find out whether subjective time perception could serve as a measure of cognitive workload during simulated car driving. Participants drove on a round course of a driving simulator consisting of three different environments with different levels of task demands. Drivers were required to perform a time-production task while driving the vehicle. Electrodermal activity and subjective ratings of mental workload (SWAT) were recorded simultaneously. The length of produced intervals increased significantly in more complex driving situations, as did electrodermal activity and subjective ratings of mental workload. Thus, time production is a valid indicator of cognitive involvement in simulated driving and could become a valid method to measure the current mental workload of car drivers in various traffic situations.
-
The "dual klepsydra model" (DKM) of internal time representation successfully models duration reproduction data, but relations between the DKM-based parameter kappa ("loss rate") and procedural variables (presentation modality) or individual characteristics (cognitive indices, age, sex) remained as yet unexplored. For that purpose, were-analyzed data from an earlier time reproduction study (N = 100), using visually or acoustically presented intervals of 1-5 sec. duration. Typical values of parameter kappa were approximately 0.03-0.04 sec.(-1), corresponding to relaxation times of internal "lossy integrators" of approximately 30 sec. Significant effects of presentation modality (smaller kappa values for the visual reproduction task) and of age (greater kappa in acoustic reproduction with increasing age) were observed. Cognitive variables (working memory, general fluid reasoning, attention) and sex of participants were not associated with kappa. Cognitive functions seem to play only a minor, if any, role at the level of time representation addressed by the DKM.
-
Individuals are different 'chronotypes' with early 'larks' and late 'owls' forming the limits of a normal distribution in the population. We recently described that late chronotypes who suffer from a conflict between internal and external time ('social jetlag') suffer from more mental distress and are more likely to smoke than early chronotypes (Wittmann, Dinich, Merrow, and Roenneberg, 2006 . Social jetlag: mis-alignment of biological and social time. Chronobiology International, 23:497-509.). We performed a detailed analysis of the same database collected in 2002 comprising 134 daily smokers and 366 nonsmokers, scrutinizing the relationships between chronotype, smoking, and alcohol consumption as well as psychological well-being using a multiple mediation analysis. On average, smokers tend to be later chronotypes, report more sleep-associated psychosomatic symptoms, are more depressed, less balanced, and less vigilant. The mediation analysis suggests that only those late chronotypes who smoke and those who drink more suffer from increased psychological distress. We suggest that 'chronotype' is introduced as an additional factor in substance use, that is, when considering motives for smoking and drinking.
-
The perception of time is a fundamental part of human experience. Recent research suggests that the experience of time emerges from emotional and interoceptive (bodily) states as processed in the insular cortex. Whether there is an interaction between the conscious awareness of interoceptive states and time distortions induced by emotions has rarely been investigated so far. We aimed to address this question by the use of a retrospective time estimation task comparing two groups of participants. One group had a focus on interoceptive states and one had a focus on exteroceptive information while watching film clips depicting fear, amusement and neutral content. Main results were that attention to interoceptive processes significantly affected subjective time experience. Fear was accompanied with subjective time dilation that was more pronounced in the group with interoceptive focus, while amusement led to a quicker passage of time which was also increased by interoceptive focus. We conclude that retrospective temporal distortions are directly influenced by attention to bodily responses. These effects might crucially interact with arousal levels. Sympathetic nervous system activation affecting memory build-up might be the decisive factor influencing retrospective time judgments. Our data substantially extend former research findings underscoring the relevance of interoception for the effects of emotional states on subjective time experience.
-
Data from three experiments on serial perception of temporal intervals in the supra-second domain are reported. Sequences of short acoustic signals ("pips") separated by periods of silence were presented to the observers. Two types of time series, geometric or alternating, were used, where the modulus 1+δ of the inter-pip series and the base duration Tb (range from 1.1 to 6s) were varied as independent parameters. The observers had to judge whether the series were accelerating, decelerating, or uniform (3 paradigm), or to distinguish regular from irregular sequences (2 paradigm). "Intervals of subjective uniformity" (isus) were obtained by fitting Gaussian psychometric functions to individual subjects' responses. Progression towards longer base durations (Tb=4.4 or 6s) shifts the isus towards negative δs, i.e., accelerating series. This finding is compatible with the phenomenon of "subjective shortening" of past temporal intervals, which is naturally accounted for by the lossy integration model of internal time representation. The opposite effect observed for short durations (Tb=1.1 or 1.5s) remains unexplained by the lossy integration model, and presents a challenge for further research.
-
Boredom is an everyday experience during uneventful situations and while waiting. Such situations are typically described as unpleasant since oneself becomes the focus of awareness, and the subjective duration expands. Self-control is an individual trait that helps to cope with unpleasant situations. Hardly any systematic studies exist on real waiting situations where people's boredom and subjective time experience are assessed in relation to self-control. Therefore, we assessed 99 participants who first filled out self-report questionnaires on emotional and metacognitive control, the Scale for Experiencing Emotions (SEE) and the Temporal Metacognition Scale (TMCS). After giving a fake reason for why they had to wait, participants were shut in an empty room alone for 7.5 minutes and afterwards asked to report their impressions regarding the experienced time and reactions to the situation. Boredom was associated with the feeling of time passing more slowly and more frequent thoughts about time. The propensity to self-regulate was related to less awareness of time and to lower levels of boredom. Mediation analyses revealed that the level of boredom mediates the relationships between self-regulation processes and time perception. The results provide new insights into the association between time perception and boredom as states in a real situation and self-regulation as a trait.
-
Neuropsychological studies in brain-injured patients with aphasia and children with specific language-learning deficits have shown the dependence of language comprehension on auditory processing abilities, i.e. the detection of temporal order. An impairment of temporal-order perception can be simulated by time reversing segments of the speech signal. In our study, we investigated how different lengths of time-reversed segments in speech influenced comprehension in ten native German speakers and ten participants who had acquired German as a second language. Results show that native speakers were still able to understand the distorted speech at segment lengths of 50 ms, whereas non-native speakers only could identify sentences with reversed intervals of 32 ms duration. These differences in performance can be interpreted by different levels of semantic and lexical proficiency. Our method of temporally-distorted speech offers a new approach to assess language skills that indirectly taps into lexical and semantic competence of non-native speakers.
-
OBJECTIVE: The experience of time is strongly related to our momentary mood states. Patients with a life-threatening illness experience an extreme change in mood and suffer from psychological distress that can develop into clinically relevant psychiatric disorders, like anxiety and depression. The aim of this study was to investigate the associations among the subjective perception of time, psychological distress, and quality of life in patients with hematological malignancies. METHODS: Eighty-eight inpatients with hematological malignancies rated how fast time passes subjectively on a visual analog scale and prospectively estimated a time span of 13 min. The Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale (HADS) self-report measures of health-related quality of life (FACT-G) and spiritual well-being (FACIT-Sp) were employed to assess psychological distress and quality of life. RESULTS: Those patients who reported a lower quality of life, less spiritual well-being, and more anxiety experienced a slower passage of subjective time and overestimated the 13-min time interval. SIGNIFICANCE OF RESULTS: Our interpretation of the results is that patients with a life-threatening illness who show symptoms of psychological distress draw attention away from meaningful thoughts and actions and, thus, experience time as passing more slowly. An altered sense of time can be a sign of mental suffering, which should be addressed within psycho-oncological interventions. As this is the first study to demonstrate this relation in cancer patients, further research is needed to investigate the experience of time and its relation to meaning as an issue in clinical diagnostics.
-
Some authors have suggested separate mechanisms for the processing of temporal intervals above versus below 2-3s. Given that the evidence is mixed, the present experiment was carried out as a critical test of the separate-mechanism hypothesis. Subjects reproduced five standard durations of 1-5s presented in the auditory and visual modalities. The Corsi-block test was used to assess effects of working-memory span on different interval lengths. Greater working-memory span was associated with longer reproductions of intervals of 3-5s. A factor analysis run on mean reproduced intervals revealed one modality-unspecific factor for durations of 1-2s and two modality-specific factors for longer intervals. These results are interpreted as further indications that two different processes underlie temporal reproductions of shorter and longer intervals.
-
Humans show large differences in the preferred timing of their sleep and activity. This so-called "chronotype" is largely regulated by the circadian clock. Both genetic variations in clock genes and environmental influences contribute to the distribution of chronotypes in a given population, ranging from extreme early types to extreme late types with the majority falling between these extremes. Social (e.g., school and work) schedules interfere considerably with individual sleep preferences in the majority of the population. Late chronotypes show the largest differences in sleep timing between work and free days leading to a considerable sleep debt on work days, for which they compensate on free days. The discrepancy between work and free days, between social and biological time, can be described as 'social jetlag.' Here, we explore how sleep quality and psychological wellbeing are associated with individual chronotype and/or social jetlag. A total of 501 volunteers filled out the Munich ChronoType Questionnaire (MCTQ) as well as additional questionnaires on: (i) sleep quality (SF-A), (ii) current psychological wellbeing (Basler Befindlichkeitsbogen), (iii) retrospective psychological wellbeing over the past week (POMS), and (iv) consumption of stimulants (e.g., caffeine, nicotine, and alcohol). Associations of chronotype, wellbeing, and stimulant consumption are strongest in teenagers and young adults up to age 25 yrs. The most striking correlation exists between chronotype and smoking, which is significantly higher in late chronotypes of all ages (except for those in retirement). We show these correlations are most probably a consequence of social jetlag, i.e., the discrepancies between social and biological timing rather than a simple association to different chronotypes. Our results strongly suggest that work (and school) schedules should be adapted to chronotype whenever possible.
-
The present paper investigates the effects of age, sex, and cognitive factors on temporal-order perception. Nine temporal-order tasks were employed using two and four stimuli presented in the auditory and visual modalities. Significantly increased temporal-order thresholds (TOT) in the elderly were found for almost all tasks, while sex differences were only observed for two tasks. Multiple regression analyses show that the performance on most temporal-order tasks can be predicted by cognitive factors, such as speed of fluid reasoning, short-term memory, and attention. However, age was a significant predictor of TOT in three tasks using visual stimuli. We conclude (1) that age-related differences can often be attributed to cognitive factors involved in temporal-order perception, and (2) that the concept of temporal-order perception is more complex than implied by the current models.
-
We assessed the effect of size and localization of a brain lesion on patients' abilities to perceive the temporal order of two acoustic stimuli. In those patients who had performed with impaired order thresholds, local overlaps of lesions as analyzed with CT were found in specific left-hemispheric regions of the temporal and parietal lobe. However, a moderate association of lesion size and temporal-order threshold was found among all brain-injured patients (n = 30), a correlation that was most pronounced in patients with right-hemispheric lesions. This non-specific effect of lesion size has to be discussed critically with respect to behavioral findings of an association between temporal-processing abilities and language competence.
-
Action of a hallucinogenic substance, psilocybin, on internal time representation was investigated in two double-blind, placebo-controlled studies: Experiment 1 with 12 subjects and graded doses, and Experiment 2 with 9 subjects and a very low dose. The task consisted in repeated reproductions of time intervals in the range from 1.5 to 5s. The effects were assessed by parameter kappa of the 'dual klepsydra' model of internal time representation, fitted to individual response data and intra-individually normalized with respect to initial values. The estimates kappa were in the same order of magnitude as in earlier studies. In both experiments, kappa was significantly increased by psilocybin at 90 min from the drug intake, indicating a higher loss rate of the internal duration representation. These findings are tentatively linked to qualitative alterations of subjective time in altered states of consciousness.
-
Intuitively, being aware of one's inner processes to move should be crucial for the control of voluntary movements. However, research findings suggest that we are not always aware of the processes leading to movement execution. The present study investigated induced first-person access to inner processes of movement initiation and the underlying brain activities which contribute to the emergence of voluntary movement. Moreover, we investigated differences in task performance between mindfulness meditators and non-meditators while assuming that meditators are more experienced in attending to their inner processes. Two Libet-type tasks were performed; one in which participants were asked to press a button at a moment of their own decision, and the other one in which participants' attention was directed towards their inner processes of decision making regarding the intended movement which lead them to press the button. Meditators revealed a consistent readiness potential (RP) between the two tasks with correlations between the subjective intention time to act and the slope of the early RP. However, non-meditators did not show this consistency. Instead, elicited introspection of inner processes of movement initiation changed early brain activity that is related to voluntary movement processes. Our findings suggest that compared to non-meditators, meditators are more able to access the emergence of negative deflections of slow cortical potentials (SCPs), which could have fundamental effects on initiating a voluntary movement with awareness.
-
Altered states of consciousness lead to profound changes in the sense of self, time and space. We assessed how these changes were related to sexual responsiveness during sex. 116 subjects reported (a) intensity of awareness concerning body, space and time, and (b) satisfaction, desire, arousal, and orgasm occurrence. We differentiated vaginal intercourse orgasm from noncoital orgasm. Female vaginal intercourse orgasm was further differentiated as with or without concurrent clitoral masturbation. Overall, sexual responsiveness was related to greater body awareness and lesser time and space awareness. Satisfaction, desire, and arousal were especially associated with less time awareness in women. Female orgasms during vaginal intercourse were related to greater body awareness and lesser time awareness, but noncoital orgasms were unrelated. Our findings provide empirical support for the hypotheses that altered states of consciousness with attentional absorption are strongly related to sexual responsiveness in women, and to a lesser extent in men.
-
Research findings link rolandic beta-band activity to voluntary movements, but a linkage with the decision time to move remains unknown. We found that beta-band (16-28Hz) activity shortly before the movement onset is relevant for the decision time to move: the more pronounced the decrease in beta-band synchronization, the earlier the subjective experience of the decision to move. The linkage was relevant regarding 'decision', but not regarding 'intention' timing that has been often applied in the study of free will. Our findings suggest that oscillatory neural activity in the beta-band is an important neural signature pertaining to the subjective experience of making a decision to move.
Erkunden
Eintragsart
- Zeitschriftenartikel (38)
Sprache
- Englisch (38)
Thema
- Female
- Abstinence (1)
- Acoustic Stimulation (4)
- Acoustic Stimulation/methods (5)
- Adolescent (9)
- Adult (34)
- Affect/drug effects (1)
- Age Distribution (1)
- Age Factors (2)
- Aged (11)
- Aged, 80 and over (7)
- aging (1)
- Aging/*physiology (1)
- Aging/*physiology/*psychology (1)
- Aging/psychology (1)
- Aging/*psychology (1)
- Alcohol Drinking/*psychology (1)
- *Alcoholic Beverages (1)
- Altered states of consciousness (2)
- Amphetamine-Related Disorders/*diagnosis/*physiopathology (1)
- Amphetamine-Related Disorders/physiopathology/psychology/rehabilitation (1)
- Analysis of Variance (5)
- Anxiety (1)
- Anxiety/diagnosis/etiology (1)
- Aphasia/*physiopathology (1)
- Aphasia/physiopathology/psychology (1)
- Arousal/*drug effects (1)
- *Attention (1)
- Attention/physiology (3)
- *Auditory Perception/physiology (1)
- Auditory Perception/*physiology (5)
- Auditory Perception/physiology (1)
- Auditory Threshold/*physiology (1)
- *Automobile Driving (1)
- *Automobiles (1)
- Awakening (1)
- *Awareness (1)
- Awareness/*physiology (5)
- Awareness/physiology (1)
- balanced time perspective (1)
- Ball Drawing Test (1)
- Bed Rest (1)
- Beta oscillation (1)
- *Beta Rhythm (1)
- Biological Clocks (1)
- Body boundaries (1)
- *Boredom (1)
- Boredom (1)
- Brain/blood supply/pathology (1)
- Brain/*drug effects/physiology (1)
- Brain Injuries/pathology/*physiopathology (1)
- Brain Injury, Chronic/physiopathology/*psychology (1)
- Brain Mapping (4)
- *Brain Mapping (1)
- Brain/*physiology (2)
- Brain/physiology (1)
- *Brain/physiology (1)
- Brain/physiopathology (1)
- Brain Waves/*physiology (1)
- *Buddhism (1)
- Case-Control Studies (1)
- Central Nervous System Depressants/pharmacology (1)
- Cerebral Cortex/pathology/*physiopathology (1)
- Cerebral Cortex/*physiology (3)
- Cerebral Cortex/physiology (1)
- Cerebral Cortex/physiopathology (2)
- Cerebral Cortex/*physiopathology (2)
- Cerebral Infarction/physiopathology/psychology (1)
- Child (1)
- Child, Preschool (1)
- Chronobiology Phenomena (1)
- *Circadian Rhythm/drug effects (1)
- Cocaine dependence (1)
- Cocaine-Related Disorders/*physiopathology (1)
- *Cognition (3)
- Cognition/*drug effects/physiology (1)
- Cognition/physiology (1)
- Coitus/*physiology (1)
- *Comprehension/physiology (1)
- Comprehension/*physiology (1)
- Consciousness Disorders/chemically induced (1)
- Consciousness/*drug effects (1)
- Consciousness/*physiology (1)
- *Consciousness/physiology (1)
- Contingent negative variation (1)
- contingent negative variation (CNV) (1)
- Contingent Negative Variation/*physiology (3)
- Contingent Negative Variation/physiology (1)
- Corpus Striatum/*physiopathology (1)
- Cross-Over Studies (1)
- Cross-Sectional Studies (1)
- Current source density (1)
- Decision (1)
- Decision Making (1)
- *Decision Making (1)
- Decision Making/*physiology (1)
- Depersonalization/chemically induced (1)
- Depression/diagnosis/etiology (1)
- Discrimination, Psychological (1)
- *Discrimination, Psychological/physiology (1)
- Discrimination, Psychological/*physiology (2)
- Distant healing (1)
- Distant intention (1)
- Dominance, Cerebral/physiology (1)
- Dopamine Agonists/adverse effects/*pharmacology (1)
- Dose-Response Relationship, Drug (2)
- Double-Blind Method (1)
- Duration discrimination (1)
- duration reproduction (1)
- Education (1)
- Electrocardiography (1)
- Electroencephalography (2)
- *Electroencephalography (1)
- Electroencephalography/*methods (1)
- Emotion (1)
- emotion regulation (1)
- *Emotional Regulation/physiology (1)
- *Emotions (1)
- Emotions (1)
- Emotions/*physiology (2)
- Emotions/physiology (1)
- Ethanol/pharmacology (1)
- Ethanol/*pharmacology (1)
- Event-related desynchronization (1)
- Event-related potential (1)
- Evoked Potentials/*physiology (1)
- Evoked Potentials/physiology (1)
- Evoked Potentials, Visual/*physiology (1)
- Factor Analysis, Statistical (1)
- *Fingers (1)
- Floatation-REST (1)
- FMI (1)
- Follow-Up Studies (1)
- Frontal Lobe/physiopathology (1)
- Functional Laterality (1)
- Functional Laterality/physiology (1)
- Functional magnetic resonance imaging (1)
- Galvanic Skin Response (2)
- *Gender Identity (1)
- Hallucinogens/*administration & dosage (1)
- Hallucinogens/adverse effects/*pharmacology (1)
- Heart/*physiology (1)
- Heart Rate/physiology (1)
- *Heart Rate/physiology (1)
- heartbeat-counting task (1)
- heartbeat-evoked potential (HEP) (1)
- Hematologic Neoplasms/diagnosis/*psychology (1)
- Hospitalization (1)
- Humans (38)
- Image Processing, Computer-Assisted (1)
- Impulsive Behavior/*pathology/*physiopathology (1)
- *Intention (2)
- Intention (2)
- *Interoception/physiology (1)
- Interval of subjective uniformity (1)
- Jet Lag Syndrome/diagnosis/*physiopathology (1)
- Judgment/*drug effects/physiology (1)
- Judgment/*physiology (1)
- Judgment/physiology (1)
- Karnofsky Performance Status (1)
- Language (1)
- *Language (1)
- *Learning (1)
- Libet experiment (1)
- Likelihood Functions (2)
- Magnetic Resonance Imaging (5)
- Male (37)
- Meditation (3)
- *Meditation (1)
- Meditation/*methods (1)
- Meditation/methods/*psychology (1)
- Memory/drug effects (1)
- *Memory, Short-Term (1)
- Memory, Short-Term/*physiology (1)
- Mental Processes/drug effects/physiology (1)
- Mental Processes/*physiology (1)
- Methamphetamine (2)
- Methamphetamine dependence (1)
- Middle Aged (19)
- mindfulness (1)
- *Mindfulness (1)
- Mindfulness (1)
- Models, Neurological (1)
- Models, Psychological (1)
- *Motion Sickness/drug therapy (1)
- Motivation (1)
- *Movement (1)
- Movement/*physiology (1)
- Movement/physiology (1)
- Multivariate Analysis (1)
- Nausea/etiology (1)
- Neostriatum/*physiopathology (1)
- Neuroimaging (1)
- Neuropsychological Tests (4)
- Nicotine/pharmacology (1)
- Optical Illusions/*physiology (1)
- Orgasm (1)
- Orgasm/*physiology (1)
- Oxygen/blood (1)
- *Parapsychology (1)
- passage of time (1)
- Pattern Recognition, Physiological (1)
- Perception/*physiology (1)
- Periodicity (1)
- Personal Satisfaction (1)
- *Personality (1)
- Personality Inventory (1)
- *Personality Tests (1)
- Perspectival contraction of time (1)
- Phenotype (1)
- *Photic Stimulation (1)
- Photic Stimulation (3)
- Photic Stimulation/methods (2)
- Photoperiod (1)
- *Placebo Effect (1)
- Portugal (1)
- Predictive Value of Tests (1)
- Pressure (1)
- Prognosis (1)
- Proportional Hazards Models (1)
- Prospective Studies (1)
- Psilocybin/*administration & dosage (1)
- Psilocybin/adverse effects/*pharmacology (1)
- Psychoacoustics (1)
- Psychological absorption (1)
- Psychomotor Performance (1)
- Psychomotor Performance/*drug effects (1)
- Psychomotor Performance/*physiology (1)
- Psychophysics/methods (1)
- *Quality of Life (1)
- Reaction Time (2)
- *Reaction Time (1)
- Reaction time (1)
- Reaction Time/physiology (2)
- Reaction Time/*physiology (1)
- Readiness potential (1)
- Recurrence (3)
- Reference Values (2)
- Regression Analysis (1)
- *Reinforcement, Psychology (1)
- Relapse (2)
- Relaxation (1)
- Reproducibility of Results (3)
- Respiration (1)
- Rest/physiology (1)
- Retrospective Studies (1)
- *Reward (1)
- Reward (2)
- Risk prediction (1)
- *Risk-Taking (1)
- ROC Curve (1)
- Self-control (1)
- Self-Control/*psychology (1)
- Self-regulation (1)
- Self Report (3)
- Self-transcendence (1)
- Sensory Thresholds/physiology (2)
- Serotonin 5-HT1 Receptor Agonists (1)
- Serotonin 5-HT2 Receptor Agonists (1)
- *Sex Characteristics (2)
- Sex Factors (1)
- Sexual Behavior/*physiology (1)
- Sexual Partners (1)
- Sexual responsiveness (1)
- Sleep/*drug effects (1)
- Slow cortical potential (1)
- Smoking/*psychology (1)
- Space Perception/drug effects (1)
- Speech Discrimination Tests (1)
- Speech Perception/*physiology (1)
- Spirituality (1)
- Statistics, Nonparametric (2)
- Stomach (1)
- *Stress, Psychological (1)
- Stress, Psychological/*psychology (1)
- Striatum (1)
- Subjective shortening (1)
- Substance-Related Disorders/physiopathology (1)
- Substance-Related Disorders/*physiopathology/*psychology (1)
- Surveys and Questionnaires (6)
- Survival Analysis (1)
- Sympathetic Nervous System/physiology (1)
- Task Performance and Analysis (2)
- Test Anxiety Scale (1)
- time awareness (1)
- Time Factors (11)
- *Time Perception (5)
- Time perception (3)
- time perception (1)
- Time Perception/*drug effects (1)
- Time Perception/*drug effects/physiology (1)
- *Time Perception/physiology (2)
- Time Perception/*physiology (9)
- time perspective (1)
- Tomography Scanners, X-Ray Computed (1)
- Touch Perception/*physiology (1)
- Uniformity illusion (1)
- Verbal Behavior/*physiology (1)
- Visual Perception/*physiology (2)
- Visual Perception/physiology (1)
- Volition (2)
- Volition/*physiology (1)
- Volition/physiology (1)
- Waiting situation (1)
- Wakefulness (3)
- *Wakefulness/physiology (1)
- *Wine (1)
- Work Schedule Tolerance (1)
- *Workload (1)
- Young Adult (14)
- ZTPI (1)