Ihre Suche
Ergebnisse 36 Einträge
-
Because of its abstract nature, worrying might function as an avoidance response in order to cognitively disengage from fearful imagery. The present functional magnetic resonance imaging study investigated neural correlates of aversive imagery and their association with worry tendencies, as measured by the Penn State Worry Questionnaire (PSWQ). Nineteen healthy women first viewed, and subsequently imagined pictures from two categories, 'threat' and 'happiness'. Worry tendencies were negatively correlated with brain activation in the anterior cingulate cortex, the prefrontal cortex (dorsolateral, dorsomedial, ventrolateral), the parietal cortex and the insula. These negative correlations between PSWQ scores and localized brain activation were specific for aversive imagery. Moreover, activation in the above mentioned regions was positively associated with the experienced vividness of both pleasant and unpleasant mental pictures. As the identified brain regions are involved in emotion regulation, vivid imagery and memory retrieval, a lowered activity in high PSWQ scorers might be associated with cognitive disengagement from aversive imagery as well as insufficient refresh rates of mental pictures. Our preliminary findings encourage future imagery studies on generalized anxiety disorder patients, as one of the main symptoms of this disorder is excessive worrying.
-
Facilitated detection of threatening visual cues is thought to be adaptive. In theory, detection of threat cues should activate the amygdala independently from allocation of attention. However, previous studies using emotional facial expressions as well as phobic cues yielded contradictory results. We used fMRI to examine whether the allocation of attention to components of superimposed spider and bird displays modulates amygdala activation. Nineteen spider-phobic women were instructed to identify either a moving or a stationary animal in briefly presented double-exposure displays. Amygdala activation followed a dose-response relationship: Compared to congruent neutral displays (two birds), amygdala activation was most pronounced in response to congruent phobic displays (two spiders) and less but still significant in response to mixed displays (spider and bird) when attention was focused on the phobic component. When attention was focused on the neutral component, mixed displays did not result in significant amygdala activation. This was confirmed in a significant parametric graduation of the amygdala activation in the order of congruent phobic displays, mixed displays with attention focus on the spider, mixed displays with focus on the bird and congruent neutral displays. These results challenge the notion that amygdala activation in response to briefly presented phobic cues is independent from attention.
-
The present paper investigates the effects of age, sex, and cognitive factors on temporal-order perception. Nine temporal-order tasks were employed using two and four stimuli presented in the auditory and visual modalities. Significantly increased temporal-order thresholds (TOT) in the elderly were found for almost all tasks, while sex differences were only observed for two tasks. Multiple regression analyses show that the performance on most temporal-order tasks can be predicted by cognitive factors, such as speed of fluid reasoning, short-term memory, and attention. However, age was a significant predictor of TOT in three tasks using visual stimuli. We conclude (1) that age-related differences can often be attributed to cognitive factors involved in temporal-order perception, and (2) that the concept of temporal-order perception is more complex than implied by the current models.
-
Neurofunctional mechanisms underlying cognitive behavior therapy (CBT) are still not clearly understood. This functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) study focused on changes in brain activation as a result of one-session CBT in patients suffering from spider phobia. Twenty-six female spider phobics and 25 non-phobic subjects were presented with spider pictures, generally disgust-inducing, generally fear-inducing and affectively neutral scenes in an initial fMRI session. Afterwards, the patients were randomly assigned to either a therapy group (TG) or a waiting list group (WG). The scans were repeated one week after the treatment or after a one-week waiting period. Relative to the non-phobic participants, the patients displayed increased activation in the amygdala and the fusiform gyrus as well as decreased activation in the medial orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) during the first exposure. The therapy effect consisted of increased medial OFC activity in the TG relative to the WG. Further, therapy-related reductions in experienced somatic anxiety symptoms were positively correlated with activation decreases in the amygdala and the insula. We conclude that successful treatment of spider phobia is primarily accompanied by functional changes of the medial OFC. This brain region is crucial for the self-regulation of emotions and the relearning of stimulus-reinforcement associations.
-
Inconsistent findings from several functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies on fear and disgust raise the question which brain regions are relatively specialized and which are general in the processing of these basic emotions. Some of these inconsistencies could partially be due to inter-individual differences in the experience of the applied emotional stimuli. In the present study, we therefore correlated the participants' individual online reports of fear and disgust with their hemodynamic responses towards each of the fear- and disgust-inducing scenes. Sixty six participants (32 females) took part in the fMRI study. In an event-related design, they saw 50 pictures with different emotional impact (10 neutral, 20 disgust-inducing, 20 fear-inducing). Pictures were presented for 4 s and participants rated each picture online - just after the presentation - on the dimensions disgust and fear among others. The results indicate that the processing of disgust- and fear-inducing pictures involves similar as well as distinct brain regions. Both emotional stimulus categories resulted in activations in the extended occipital cortex, in the prefrontal cortex, and in the amygdala. However, insula activations were only significantly correlated with subjective ratings of disgust, pointing to a specific role of this brain structure in the processing of disgust.
-
If we observe an ambiguous figure, our percept is unstable and alternates between the possible interpretations. Periodically interrupting the presentation sizably modulates the spontaneous reversal rate. We here studied event-related potential (ERP) correlates of the neural processes underlying these strong modulations. An ambiguous Necker stimulus was presented discontinuously with four randomly varying interstimulus intervals (ISI; 14, 43, 130, 390 ms) while participants indicated perceptual reversals. EEG was selectively averaged with respect to the participants' percept and ISI. ERP traces varied markedly between ISIs. A simple model explained a major part of this variation and showed that the ISI-dependent ERP modulation occurs after disambiguation has already taken place. We suggest that perceptual stability (or reversal) depends on a system state, slowly changing from one reversal to the next. ISI can shift this state on a scale between stability and instability.
-
The observation of an ambiguous figure leads to spontaneous perceptual reversals while the observed picture stays unchanged. Some ERP studies on ambiguous figures report a P300-like component correlated with perceptual reversals supporting a top-down explanation, while other studies found early visual ERP components supporting a bottom-up explanation. Based on an experimental paradigm that permits a high temporal resolution of the endogenous reversal event, we compared endogenous Necker-cube reversals with exogenously-induced reversals of unambiguous cube variants. For both reversal types, we found a chain of ERP components with the following characteristics: (1) An early occipital ERP component (130 ms) is restricted to endogenous reversals. (2) All subsequent components also appear with exogenously-induced reversals, however 40-90 ms earlier than their endogenous counterparts. (3) The latency difference between reversal types is also reflected in the timing of manual reactions, which occur 100-130 ms after P300-like components. The results suggest that the P300-like component is the same as found in other ERP studies on ambiguous figures. This component does not reflect the reversal per se, but rather its cognitive analysis, 300 ms after a change of the representation in early visual areas. The presented ERP chains integrate the different ERP results and allow to pinpoint the steps where top-down mechanisms begin to exert their influence.
-
The stress hormone cortisol is known to influence declarative memory and associative learning. In animals, stress has often been reported to have opposing effects on memory and learning in males and females. In humans, the effects of cortisol have mainly been studied at the behavioral level. The aim of the present experiment was to characterize the effects of a single cortisol dose (30 mg) on the hemodynamic correlates of fear conditioning. In a double-blind group comparison study subjects (17 females and 17 males) received 30 mg cortisol or placebo orally before participating in a discriminative fear conditioning paradigm. Results revealed that cortisol impaired electrodermal signs of learning (the first interval response) in males, while no conditioned SCRs emerged for the females independent of treatment. fMRI results showed that cortisol reduced activity for the CS+ > CS- comparison in the anterior cingulate, the lateral orbitofrontal cortex and the medial prefrontal cortex in males. Opposite findings (increase in these regions under cortisol) were detected in females. In addition, cortisol reduced the habituation in the CS+ > CS- contrast in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex independent of sex. Finally, cortisol also modified the response to the electric shock (the UCS) by enhancing the activity of the anterior as well as the posterior cingulate. In sum, these findings demonstrate that in humans cortisol mostly influences prefrontal brain activation during fear conditioning and that these effects appear to be modulated by sex.
-
The present functional magnetic resonance imaging study investigated the fear and disgust reactivity of patients suffering from spider phobia. Ten phobics and 13 control subjects were scanned while viewing alternating blocks of phobia-relevant, generally fear-inducing, disgust-inducing and affectively neutral pictures. The patient group rated the spider pictures as being more disgust and fear evoking than the control group, and showed greater activation of the visual association cortex, the amygdalae, the right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and the right hippocampus. Specific phobia-related activation occurred in the supplementary motor area. The patients also showed greater amygdala activation during the presentation of generally disgust- and fear-inducing pictures. This points to an elevated sensitivity to repulsive and threatening stimuli in spider phobics and implicates the amygdala as a crucial neural substrate.
-
The question to what extent emotion-related brain activation depends upon the presentation design (block design vs. event-related design) and the stimulus type (scene pictures vs. pictures with facial mimic) has hardly been addressed in previous functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) research. In the present fMRI experiment, 40 right-handed subjects viewed pictures with fear-inducing and disgust-inducing content as well as facial expressions of fear and disgust. Pictures of neutral objects and neutral facial mimic were used as control stimuli. The pictures were presented in a block design for half of the subjects; the other half viewed the same stimuli as singular events in randomized sequence. The participants had been instructed to passively view the pictures. Disgust-evoking scenes provoked activation in the amygdala, the insula and the orbitofrontal cortex (OFC). This applied to the blocked as well as to the event-related design. Fear-relevant scenes were associated with activity in the insula, the OFC and the middle temporal gyri in the event-related design. The presentation in a block design only led to activation in the middle temporal gyri. Facial expressions of disgust and fear did not trigger significant activation neither in the blocked nor event-related design. This surprising outcome may be a result of context and task effects. The face stimuli which were presented together with the more complex scenes in a passive viewing paradigm possibly were not salient enough to trigger emotional processing.
-
This functional magnetic resonance imaging study investigated the disgust- and fear-reactivity of patients suffering from obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD). Ten OCD patients were scanned while viewing blocks of pictures showing OCD triggers from their personal environment and OCD-irrelevant disgust-inducing, fear-inducing and neutral scenes. Afterwards, the patients rated the intensity of the induced disgust, fear and OCD symptoms. The responses were compared with those of 10 healthy control subjects. The disorder-relevant pictures provoked intense OCD symptoms in the clinical group associated with increased activation in the bilateral prefrontal cortex, the left insula, the right supramarginal gyrus, the left caudate nucleus and the right thalamus. The patients gave higher disgust and fear ratings than the controls for all aversive picture categories. Neural responses towards the disorder-irrelevant disgusting and fear-inducing material included more pronounced insula activation in patients than controls. Summarizing, photos of individual OCD-triggers are an effective means of symptom provocation and activation of the fronto-striato-thalamo-parietal network. The increased insular reactivity of OCD patients during all aversive picture conditions might mirror their susceptibility to experience negative somatic states.
-
The major goal of the present functional magnetic resonance imaging study was to investigate the influence of disgust sensitivity on hemodynamic responses during disgust induction. Fifteen subjects viewed three different film excerpts (duration: 135 s each) with disgust-evoking, threatening and neutral content. The films were presented in a block design with four repetitions of each condition. Afterwards, subjects gave affective ratings for the films and answered the questionnaire for the assessment of disgust sensitivity (QADS, []). The subjects' overall disgust sensitivity was positively related to their experienced disgust, as well as to their prefrontal cortex activation during the disgust condition. Further, there was a positive correlation between subjects' scores on the QADS subscale spoilage/decay and their amygdala activation (r=0.76). This was reasonable since the disgust film clip depicted a cockroach-invasion and the subscale spoilage/decay contains, among others, an item asking for disgust towards cockroaches. The study stresses, in accordance to previous studies, the importance of considering personality traits when studying affective responses in fMRI studies.
-
Functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies consistently demonstrate an enhanced activation of the visual cortex in reaction to emotionally salient visual stimuli. This increase of activation is probably modulated by top-down processes, that are initiated in emotion processing structures, specifically the amygdala and the orbitofrontal cortex. In the present fMRI study, a differential fear conditioning paradigm was applied to investigate this assumed modulation. Hemodynamic responses towards a neutral visual stimulus (CS+) predicting an electrical stimulation (UCS) were compared with responses towards a neutral and unpaired stimulus (CS-). Thereby, particularly the time courses of neural responses were considered. Skin conductance measures were concurrently recorded. Our results show that the differentiation between CS+ and CS- within the amygdala and the extended visual cortex was accomplished during a late acquisition phase. In the orbitofrontal cortex the differentiation occurred at an earlier stage and was then sustained throughout acquisition. It is suggested that these altering activation patterns are reflecting different phases of learning, integrating the analyzed regions to varying degrees. Additionally, the results indicate that statistical analyses comprising a temporal variation of hemodynamic responses are more likely to detect amygdala activation.
-
We examined whether males and females differ in the intensity and laterality of their hemodynamic responses towards visual disgust and fear stimuli. Forty-one female, and 51 male subjects viewed disgust-inducing, fear-inducing and neutral pictures in an fMRI block design. Self-report data indicated that the target emotions had been elicited successfully with women responding stronger than men. While viewing the fear pictures, which depicted attacks by humans or animals, men exhibited greater activation in the bilateral amygdala and the left fusiform gyrus than women. This response pattern may reflect greater attention from males to cues of aggression in their environment. Further, the lateralization of brain activation was comparable in the two genders during both aversive picture conditions.
-
Although it is known that there are fundamental personality differences in the behavioral responses to emotional stimuli, traits have scarcely been investigated in this context by means of functional imaging studies. To maximize the variance with respect to personality, the authors tested 12 control subjects and 12 subjects who had sadomasochistic experiences with respect to the relationship between J. A. Gray's (1970) personality dimensions, the behavioral approach system (BAS) and the behavioral inhibition system (BIS), and brain activity in regions of interest. The BIS was associated with activity in numerous brain areas in response to fear, disgust, and erotic visual stimuli, whereas few associations could he detected between the BAS and brain activity in response to disgust and erotic stimuli.
-
The majority of neuroimaging studies on affective processing have indicated that there are specific brain structures, which are selectively responsive to fear and disgust. Whereas the amygdala is assumed to be fear-related, the insular cortex is most likely involved in disgust processing. Since these findings are mainly a result of studies focusing exclusively either on fear, or on disgust, but rarely on both emotions together, the present experiment explored the neural effects of viewing disgusting and fear-inducing pictures in contrast to neutral pictures. This was done by means of functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) with 19 subjects (nine males, ten females), who also gave affective ratings for the presented pictures. The fear and the disgust pictures were able to induce the target emotions and they received comparable valence and arousal ratings. The processing of both aversive picture types was associated with an increased brain activation in the occipital-temporal lobe, in the prefrontal cortex, and in the thalamus. The amygdala was significantly activated by disgusting, but not by fear-inducing, pictures. Thus, our data are in contrast with the idea of highly emotion-specific brain structures and rather suggest the existence of a common affective circuit.
Erkunden
Eintragsart
- Zeitschriftenartikel (36)
Sprache
- Englisch (36)
Thema
- Photic Stimulation
- Acoustic Stimulation (2)
- action mapping (1)
- action observation (1)
- Adaptation, Physiological/physiology (1)
- Adolescent (3)
- Adult (33)
- Age Factors (1)
- Aged (2)
- Aged, 80 and over (2)
- Aging/*psychology (1)
- Algorithms (1)
- altered states of consciousness (1)
- Ambiguous figures, Necker cube, Multistable perception, EEG, ERPs, Bayesian inference (1)
- Amygdala/anatomy & histology/blood supply/physiology (1)
- Amygdala/blood supply (1)
- Amygdala/blood supply/physiology (1)
- Amygdala/*blood supply/*physiology (1)
- Amygdala/pathology (1)
- Amygdala/*physiology (1)
- Amygdala/physiology (2)
- Amygdala/*physiopathology (1)
- Analysis of Variance (4)
- Animals (3)
- Anxiety Disorders/physiopathology/*psychology (1)
- Anxiety/psychology (1)
- *Arousal (1)
- Arousal/physiology (2)
- Asperger Syndrome/diagnosis/*psychology (1)
- Association Learning/*physiology (1)
- *Attention (2)
- Attention (1)
- Attention/*physiology (2)
- Attention/physiology (1)
- *Attitude (1)
- Awareness (1)
- Awareness/physiology (1)
- Awareness/*physiology (1)
- Behavior, Addictive/*physiopathology/psychology (1)
- Blinking/*physiology (1)
- Brain (2)
- Brain/anatomy & histology/*physiology (1)
- Brain/*blood supply/*physiology (1)
- *Brain Mapping (2)
- Brain Mapping (10)
- Brain/*physiology (4)
- Brain/physiology (2)
- Brain/*physiopathology (2)
- Case-Control Studies (1)
- Cerebral Cortex/blood supply/physiology (1)
- Cerebral Cortex/physiology (1)
- Cerebral Cortex/*physiology (2)
- Cerebrovascular Circulation (2)
- Cerebrovascular Circulation/physiology (2)
- Cerebrovascular Circulation/*physiology (1)
- Child, Preschool (1)
- *Cognition (2)
- Cognition/physiology (1)
- *Cognitive Behavioral Therapy (1)
- color effects (1)
- Conditioning, Classical/*physiology (2)
- Conditioning, Psychological (1)
- Conditioning, Psychological/*drug effects (1)
- Conditioning, Psychological/*drug effects/physiology (1)
- Conditioning, Psychological/*physiology (2)
- Conditioning, Psychological/physiology (1)
- Consciousness (1)
- Contraception Behavior/psychology (1)
- Contraceptives, Oral/*pharmacology/therapeutic use (1)
- Cues (2)
- *Cues (1)
- Dancing/physiology (1)
- Data Interpretation, Statistical (2)
- *Desensitization, Psychologic (1)
- Discrimination, Psychological/*physiology (1)
- Double-Blind Method (4)
- Electric Stimulation (5)
- Electroencephalography (3)
- *Electroencephalography (1)
- Electromyography (1)
- Electrophysiology (1)
- *Emotions (1)
- Emotions (1)
- Emotions/*physiology (8)
- Emotions/physiology (2)
- Erotica (1)
- Event-Related Potentials, P300/*physiology (1)
- Evoked Potentials (1)
- Evoked Potentials, Visual (2)
- Evoked Potentials, Visual/*physiology (2)
- Expressed Emotion (1)
- Extinction, Psychological/*physiology (2)
- Fear (1)
- Fear/*drug effects (1)
- Fear/*drug effects/physiology (1)
- Fear/*physiology (8)
- Fear/*psychology (2)
- Fear/psychology (1)
- Female (33)
- Fixation, Ocular/physiology (1)
- fMRI (1)
- Foot/physiology (1)
- *Form Perception (1)
- Form Perception/*physiology (1)
- Fourier Analysis (1)
- Frontal Lobe/blood supply (1)
- Frontal Lobe/*physiology (1)
- Frontal Lobe/physiology (1)
- Functional Laterality (1)
- Functional Laterality/physiology (4)
- Functional Neuroimaging (1)
- Galvanic Skin Response (1)
- Galvanic Skin Response/*physiology (2)
- Galvanic Skin Response/physiology (4)
- Ganzfeld (1)
- Goals (1)
- Hand/physiology (1)
- Hemodynamics (1)
- Hemodynamics/physiology (3)
- Hemodynamics/*physiology (5)
- Hippocampus/physiology (1)
- Humans (36)
- Hydrocortisone/analysis/*metabolism (1)
- Hydrocortisone/*pharmacology (2)
- Hydrocortisone/*physiology (1)
- Image Processing, Computer-Assisted (10)
- *Imagination (1)
- Imagination/*physiology (1)
- Individuality (1)
- Inhibition, Psychological (1)
- Judgment/*physiology (1)
- Learning/*drug effects/physiology (1)
- Learning/physiology (1)
- Linear Models (2)
- Magnetic Resonance Imaging (23)
- Magnetic Resonance Imaging/methods (1)
- Male (27)
- *Memory, Short-Term (1)
- Middle Aged (4)
- Motion Perception/*physiology (2)
- *Motivation (1)
- Motivation/*physiology (1)
- Motor Activity/*physiology (1)
- Motor Cortex/*physiology (1)
- motor imagery (1)
- motor simulation (1)
- Movement/*physiology (1)
- Nerve Net/*physiology (2)
- Neural Pathways/*physiology (1)
- Neuropsychological Tests (3)
- Nucleus Accumbens/physiology (1)
- Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder/*physiopathology/psychology (1)
- Occipital Lobe/physiology (1)
- Optical Illusions/physiology (1)
- Oxygen (1)
- Oxygen/blood (3)
- Parietal Lobe/*physiology (1)
- Pattern Recognition, Visual (1)
- Pattern Recognition, Visual/*physiology (1)
- perceptual deprivation (1)
- Personality (1)
- Personality Assessment (1)
- Phobic Disorders/pathology/*psychology/*therapy (1)
- Phobic Disorders/*physiopathology (2)
- Placebos (1)
- Prefrontal Cortex/blood supply (1)
- Prefrontal Cortex/pathology (1)
- Prefrontal Cortex/*physiology (1)
- Prefrontal Cortex/physiology (1)
- Psychiatric Status Rating Scales (3)
- Psychological Theory (1)
- Psychomotor Performance/physiology (1)
- Psychophysics (1)
- Reaction Time/physiology (4)
- Recognition, Psychology/physiology (1)
- Regression Analysis (2)
- Saliva/chemistry (1)
- Self-Assessment (1)
- Sex Characteristics (3)
- *Sex Characteristics (2)
- Sex Factors (3)
- *Sexual Behavior (1)
- Sexual Behavior/*physiology (1)
- Skin/blood supply (1)
- Smoking/*physiopathology (1)
- Smoking/*physiopathology/psychology (1)
- somatotopic mapping (1)
- Spiders (2)
- *Spiders (1)
- Surveys and Questionnaires (5)
- Task Performance and Analysis (1)
- Time Factors (3)
- *Time Perception (3)
- Time Perception/*physiology (1)
- Touch Perception/*physiology (1)
- Video Recording (1)
- Vision, Binocular/physiology (1)
- Visual Cortex/*blood supply/*physiology (1)
- Visual Cortex/physiology (1)
- Visual Fields (1)
- Visual Fields/physiology (1)
- Visual Perception (2)
- Visual Perception/*physiology (5)
- Visual Perception/physiology (1)
- Young Adult (14)