Ihre Suche
Ergebnisse 229 Einträge
-
The effects of sex and stress hormones on classical fear conditioning have been subject of recent experimental studies. A correlation approach between basal cortisol concentrations and neuronal activation in fear-related structures seems to be a promising alternative approach in order to foster our understanding of how cortisol influences emotional learning. In this functional magnetic resonance imaging study, participants with varying sex hormone status (20 men, 15 women taking oral contraceptives, 15 women tested in the luteal phase) underwent an instructed fear conditioning protocol with geometrical figures as conditioned stimuli and an electrical stimulation as unconditioned stimulus. Salivary cortisol concentrations were measured and afterwards correlated with fear conditioned brain responses. Results revealed a positive correlation between basal cortisol levels and differential activation in the amygdala in men and OC women only. These results suggest that elevated endogenous cortisol levels are associated with enhanced fear anticipation depending on current sex hormone availability.
-
PURPOSE: We sought brain activity that predicts visual consciousness. METHODS: We used electroencephalography (EEG) to measure brain activity to a 1000-ms display of sine-wave gratings, oriented vertically in one eye and horizontally in the other. This display yields binocular rivalry: irregular alternations in visual consciousness between the images viewed by the eyes. We replaced both gratings with 200 ms of darkness, the gap, before showing a second display of the same rival gratings for another 1000 ms. We followed this by a 1000-ms mask then a 2000-ms inter-trial interval (ITI). Eleven participants pressed keys after the second display in numerous trials to say whether the orientation of the visible grating changed from before to after the gap or not. Each participant also responded to numerous non-rivalry trials in which the gratings had identical orientations for the two eyes and for which the orientation of both either changed physically after the gap or did not. RESULTS: We found that greater activity from lateral occipital-parietal-temporal areas about 180 ms after initial onset of rival stimuli predicted a change in visual consciousness more than 1000 ms later, on re-presentation of the rival stimuli. We also found that less activity from parietal, central, and frontal electrodes about 400 ms after initial onset of rival stimuli predicted a change in visual consciousness about 800 ms later, on re-presentation of the rival stimuli. There was no such predictive activity when the change in visual consciousness occurred because the stimuli changed physically. CONCLUSION: We found early EEG activity that predicted later visual consciousness. Predictive activity 180 ms after onset of the first display may reflect adaption of the neurons mediating visual consciousness in our displays. Predictive activity 400 ms after onset of the first display may reflect a less-reliable brain state mediating visual consciousness.
-
Pain can be modulated by several cognitive techniques, typically involving increased cognitive control and decreased sensory processing. Recently, it has been demonstrated that pain can also be attenuated by mindfulness. Here, we investigate the underlying brain mechanisms by which the state of mindfulness reduces pain. Mindfulness practitioners and controls received unpleasant electric stimuli in the functional magnetic resonance imaging scanner during a mindfulness and a control condition. Mindfulness practitioners, but not controls, were able to reduce pain unpleasantness by 22% and anticipatory anxiety by 29% during a mindful state. In the brain, this reduction was associated with decreased activation in the lateral prefrontal cortex and increased activation in the right posterior insula during stimulation and increased rostral anterior cingulate cortex activation during the anticipation of pain. These findings reveal a unique mechanism of pain modulation, comprising increased sensory processing and decreased cognitive control, and are in sharp contrast to established pain modulation mechanisms.
-
In emotional learning tasks, sex differences, stress effects and an interaction of these two moderators have often been observed. The sex hormones estradiol (E2) and progesterone (P4) vary over the menstrual cycle. We tested groups with different sex hormone status: 39 men, 30 women in the luteal phase (LU, high E2+P4) and 29 women taking oral contraceptives (OC, low E2+P4). They received either 30 mg cortisol or placebo prior to instructed differential fear conditioning consisting of neutral conditioned stimuli (CS) and an electrical stimulation (unconditioned stimulus; UCS). One figure (CS+) was paired with the UCS, the other figure (CS-) never. During extinction, no electrical stimulation was administered. Regarding fear acquisition, results showed higher skin conductance and higher brain responses to the CS+ compared to the CS- in several structures that were not modulated by cortisol or sex hormones. However, OC women exhibited higher CS+/CS- differentiations than men and LU women in the amygdala, thalamus, anterior cingulate and ventromedial prefrontal cortex during extinction. The suppression of endogenous sex hormones by OC seems to alter neuronal correlates of extinction. The observation that extinction is influenced by the current sex hormone availability is relevant for future studies and might also be clinically important.
-
The "dual klepsydra model" (DKM) of internal time representation successfully models duration reproduction data, but relations between the DKM-based parameter kappa ("loss rate") and procedural variables (presentation modality) or individual characteristics (cognitive indices, age, sex) remained as yet unexplored. For that purpose, were-analyzed data from an earlier time reproduction study (N = 100), using visually or acoustically presented intervals of 1-5 sec. duration. Typical values of parameter kappa were approximately 0.03-0.04 sec.(-1), corresponding to relaxation times of internal "lossy integrators" of approximately 30 sec. Significant effects of presentation modality (smaller kappa values for the visual reproduction task) and of age (greater kappa in acoustic reproduction with increasing age) were observed. Cognitive variables (working memory, general fluid reasoning, attention) and sex of participants were not associated with kappa. Cognitive functions seem to play only a minor, if any, role at the level of time representation addressed by the DKM.
-
An important feature of the human defense system comprises fear learning, which stress hormones can crucially modulate. However, stress hormones might influence men and women differently, in part because of interactions with sex hormones. In women, distinct stages of the menstrual cycle or the intake of oral contraceptives (OC) affect sex hormone levels. In this study, we used a differential fear conditioning paradigm with electrical stimulation as unconditioned stimulus (UCS) following one neutral stimulus (conditioned stimulus, CS+), but not another (CS-).To investigate implicit fear learning, participants were distracted from detecting the contingencies between CS and UCS. To address interaction effects of sex and stress hormones, 32 men, 30 women in the early follicular phase of the menstrual cycle (FO), 30 women in the luteal phase (LU), and 30 OC women received either 30 mg cortisol or a placebo. In the contrast CS+ minus CS-, an interaction between cortisol administration and sex hormone status emerged in the anterior parahippocampal gyrus and the hippocampus. Cortisol reduced fear learning in men, FO, and LU women, but enhanced it in OC women. Additionally, cortisol attenuated differential amygdala activation in the entire group. These results demonstrate that OC usage substantially modifies cortisol effects on emotional learning in women, particularly in memory-related medial temporal lobe regions. Further, a high dose of cortisol reduces amygdala differentiation pointing to a lowered learning ability of the defense system under high cortisol concentrations, irrespective of current sex hormone availability.
-
The unexpected return of mental clarity and memory shortly before death in patients suffering from severe psychiatric and neurologic disorders, which we have called "terminal lucidity", has been reported in the medical literature over the past 250 years, but has received little attention. We review a range of terminal lucidity cases in order to encourage investigation of the mechanisms involved and possible insights into both the neuroscience of memory and cognition at the end of life and treatment of terminal illness. These examples include case reports of patients suffering from brain abscesses, tumors, strokes, meningitis, dementia or Alzheimer's disease, schizophrenia, and affective disorders. Several of these accounts suggest that during terminal lucidity, memory and cognitive abilities may function by neurologic processes different from those of the normal brain. We expect that significant contributions to better understanding the processes involved in memory and cognition processing might be gained through in-depth studies of terminal lucidity. Studying terminal lucidity might also facilitate the development of novel therapies. In addition, increased awareness of unusual end-of-life experiences could help physicians, caregivers, and bereaved family members be prepared for encountering such experiences, and help those individuals cope with them.
-
RATIONALE: Biased processing of drug-associated stimuli is believed to be a crucial feature of addiction. Particularly, an attentional bias seems to contribute to the disorder's maintenance. Recent studies suggest differential effects for stimuli associated with the beginning (BEGIN-smoking-stimuli) or the terminal stage of the smoking ritual (END-smoking-stimuli), with the former but not the later evoking high cue-reactivity. OBJECTIVE: The current study investigated the neuronal network underlying an attentional bias to BEGIN-smoking-stimuli and END-smoking-stimuli in smokers and tested the hypothesis that the attentional bias is greater for BEGIN-smoking-stimuli. METHODS: Sixteen non-deprived smokers and 16 non-smoking controls participated in an fMRI study. Drug pictures (BEGIN-smoking-stimuli, END-smoking-stimuli) and control pictures were overlaid with geometrical figures and presented for 300 ms. Subjects had to identify picture content (identification-task) or figure orientation (distraction-task). The distraction-task was intended to demonstrate attentional bias. RESULTS: Behavioral data revealed an attentional bias to BEGIN-smoking-stimuli but not to END-smoking-stimuli in both groups. However, only smokers showed mesocorticolimbic deactivations in the distraction-task with BEGIN-smoking-stimuli. Importantly, these deactivations were significantly stronger for BEGIN- than for END-smoking-stimuli and correlated with the attentional bias score. CONCLUSIONS: Several explanations may account for missing group differences in behavioral data. Brain data suggest smokers using regulatory strategies in response to BEGIN-smoking-stimuli to prevent the elicitation of motivational responses interfering with distraction-task performance. These strategies could be reflected in the observed deactivations and might lead to a performance level in smokers that is similar to that of non-smokers.
-
BACKGROUND: Gene by environment (G×E) interaction between genetic variation in the promoter region of the serotonin transporter gene (serotonin transporter-linked polymorphic region [5-HTTLPR]) and stressful life events (SLEs) has been extensively studied in the context of depression. Recent findings suggest increased neural and endocrine stress sensitivity as a possible mechanism conveying elevated vulnerability to psychopathology. Furthermore, these G×E mediated alterations very likely reflect interrelated biological processes. METHODS: In the present functional magnetic resonance imaging study, amygdala reactivity to fearful stimuli was assessed in healthy male adults (n = 44), who were previously found to differ with regard to endocrine stress reactivity as a function of 5-HTTLPR × SLEs. Furthermore, functional connectivity between the amygdala and the hypothalamus was measured as a potential mechanism linking elevated neural and endocrine responses during stressful/threatening situations. The study sample was carefully preselected regarding 5-HTTLPR genotype and SLEs. RESULTS: We report significant G×E interaction on neural response patterns and functional amygdala-hypothalamus connectivity. Specifically, homozygous carriers of the 5-HTTLPR S' allele with a history of SLEs (S'S'/high SLEs group) displayed elevated bilateral amygdala activation in response to fearful faces. Within the same sample, a comparable G×E interaction effect has previously been demonstrated regarding increased cortisol reactivity, indicating a cross-validation of heightened biological stress sensitivity. Furthermore, S'S'/high SLEs subjects were characterized by an increased functional coupling between the right amygdala and the hypothalamus, thus indicating a potential link between neural and endocrine hyperreactivity. CONCLUSIONS: The present findings contribute to the ongoing debate on 5-HTTLPR × SLEs interaction and are discussed with respect to clinical implications.
-
The perception of action is influenced by the observer's familiarity with its movement. However, how does motor familiarity with own movement patterns modulate the visual perception of action effects? Cortical activation was examined with fMRI while 20 observers were watching videotaped point-light displays of markers on the shoulders, the right elbow, and wrist of an opposing table tennis player. The racket and ball were not displayed. Participants were asked to predict the invisible effect of the stroke, that is, the ball flight direction. Different table tennis models were used without the observers knowing and being informed in advance that some of the presented videos displayed their own movements from earlier training sessions. Prediction had to be made irrespective of the identity of the player represented by the four moving markers. Results showed that participants performed better when observing their "own" strokes. Using a region-of-interest approach, fMRI data showed that observing own videos was accompanied by stronger activation (compared to other videos) in the left angular gyrus of the inferior parietal lobe and the anterior rostral medial frontal cortex. Other videos elicited stronger activation than own videos in the left intraparietal sulcus and right supramarginal gyrus. We suggest that during action observation of motorically familiar movements, the compatibility between the observed action and the observers' motor representation is already coded in the parietal angular gyrus--in addition to the paracingulate gyrus. The activation in angular gyrus is presumably part of an action-specific effect retrieval that accompanies actor-specific prefrontal processing. The intraparietal sulcus seems to be sensitive to incongruence between observed kinematics and internal model representations, and this also influences processing in the supramarginal gyrus.
-
Patients suffering from obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD) are characterized by dysregulated neuronal processing of disorder-specific and also unspecific affective stimuli. In the present study, we investigated whether generic fear-inducing, disgust-inducing, and neutral stimuli can be decoded from brain patterns of single fMRI time samples of individual OCD patients and healthy controls. Furthermore, we tested whether differences in the underlying encoding provide information to classify subjects into groups (OCD patients or healthy controls). Two pattern classification analyses were conducted. In analysis 1, we used a classifier to decode the category of a currently viewed picture from extended fMRI patterns of single time samples (TR=3s) in individual subjects for several pairs of categories. In analysis 2, we used a searchlight approach to predict subjects' diagnostic status based on local brain patterns. In analysis 1, we obtained significant accuracies for the separation of fear-eliciting from neutral pictures in OCD patients and healthy controls. Separation of disgust-inducing from neutral pictures was significant in healthy controls. In analysis 2, we identified diagnostic information for the presence of OCD in the orbitofrontal cortex, and in the caudate nucleus. Accuracy obtained in these regions was 100% (p<10(-6)). To summarize our findings, by using multivariate pattern classification techniques we were able to identify neurobiological markers providing reliable diagnostic information about OCD. The classifier-based fMRI paradigms proposed here might be integrated in future diagnostic procedures and treatment concepts.
-
The understanding of individual differences in responses to disgusting stimuli is important to gain more insight into the development of certain psychiatric disorders. The aim of this study was to investigate conditioned disgust responses, its potential overlap with conditioned fear responses (CRs) and the influence of disgust sensitivity on blood oxygen level-dependent responses. Yet even though current studies report evidence that disgust sensitivity is a vulnerability factor, the knowledge about the underlying neural mechanisms remains very limited. Two groups were exposed either to a disgust- or a fear-conditioning paradigm. Using functional magnetic resonance imaging, we identified a conjoint activated network including the cingulate cortex, the nucleus accumbens, the orbitofrontal cortex, and the occipital cortex within the disgust- and the fear-conditioning group. Moreover, we report evidence of increased insula activation in the disgust-conditioning group. In addition, functional connectivity analysis revealed increased interconnections, most pronounced within the insula in the high disgust sensitivity group compared with the low disgust sensitivity group. The conjunction results suggest that the conditioned responses in disgust and fear conditioning recruit the same neural network, implicating that different conditioned responses of aversive learning depend on a common neural network. Increased insula activation within the disgust-conditioning group might be attributable to heightened interoceptive processes, which might be more pronounced in disgust. Finally, the findings regarding disgust sensitivity are discussed with respect to vulnerability factors for certain psychiatric disorders.
-
Remembering something that has not in fact been experienced is commonly referred to as false memory. The Deese-Roediger-McDermott (DRM) paradigm is a well-elaborated approach to this phenomenon. This study attempts to investigate the peripheral physiology of false memories induced in a visual DRM paradigm. The main research question is whether false recognition is different from true recognition in terms of accompanying physiological responses.Sixty subjects participated in the experiment, which included a study phase with visual scenes each showing a group of interrelated items in social contexts. Subjects were divided into an experimental group undergoing a classical DRM design and a control group without DRM manipulation. The control group was implemented in order to statistically control for possible biases produced by memorability differences between stimulus types. After a short retention interval, a pictorial recognition phase was conducted in the manner of a Concealed Information Test. Simultaneous recordings of electrodermal activity, respiration line length, phasic heart rate, and finger pulse waveform length were used. Results yielded a significant Group by Item Type interaction, showing that true recognition is accompanied by greater electrodermal activity than false recognition.Results are discussed in the light of Sokolov's Orienting Reflex, the Preliminary Process Theory and the Concealed Information Test. Implications and restrictions of the introduced design features are critically discussed. This study demonstrates the applicability of measures of peripheral physiology to the field of false memory research.
-
Fear acquisition and extinction are crucial mechanisms in the etiology and maintenance of anxiety disorders. Moreover, they might play a pivotal role in conveying the influence of genetic and environmental factors on the development of a (more or less) stronger proneness for, or resilience against psychopathology. There are only few insights in the neurobiology of genetically and environmentally based individual differences in fear learning and extinction. In this functional magnetic resonance imaging study, 74 healthy subjects were investigated. These were invited according to 5-HTTLPR/rs25531 (S+ vs. L(A)L(A); triallelic classification) and TPH2 (G(-703)T) (T+ vs. T-) genotype. The aim was to investigate the influence of genetic factors and traumatic life events on skin conductance responses (SCRs) and neural responses (amygdala, insula, dorsal anterior cingulate cortex (dACC) and ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC)) during acquisition and extinction learning in a differential fear conditioning paradigm. Fear acquisition was characterized by stronger late conditioned and unconditioned responses in the right insula in 5-HTTLPR S-allele carriers. During extinction traumatic life events were associated with reduced amygdala activation in S-allele carriers vs. non-carriers. Beyond that, T-allele carriers of the TPH2 (G(-703)T) polymorphism with a higher number of traumatic life events showed enhanced responsiveness in the amygdala during acquisition and in the vmPFC during extinction learning compared with non-carriers. Finally, a combined effect of the two polymorphisms with higher responses in S- and T-allele carriers was found in the dACC during extinction. The results indicate an increased expression of conditioned, but also unconditioned fear responses in the insula in 5-HTTLPR S-allele carriers. A combined effect of the two polymorphisms on dACC activation during extinction might be associated with prolonged fear expression. Gene-by-environment interactions in amygdala and vmPFC activation may reflect a neural endophenotype translating genetic and adverse environmental influences into vulnerability for or resilience against developing affective psychopathology.
-
An important feature of addiction is the high drug craving that may promote the continuation of consumption. Environmental stimuli classically conditioned to drug-intake have a strong motivational power for addicts and can elicit craving. However, addicts differ in the attitudes towards their own consumption behavior: some are content with drug taking (consonant users) whereas others are discontent (dissonant users). Such differences may be important for clinical practice because the experience of dissonance might enhance the likelihood to consider treatment. This fMRI study investigated in smokers whether these different attitudes influence subjective and neural responses to smoking stimuli. Based on self-characterization, smokers were divided into consonant and dissonant smokers. These two groups were presented smoking stimuli and neutral stimuli. Former studies have suggested differences in the impact of smoking stimuli depending on the temporal stage of the smoking ritual they are associated with. Therefore, we used stimuli associated with the beginning (BEGIN-smoking-stimuli) and stimuli associated with the terminal stage (END-smoking-stimuli) of the smoking ritual as distinct stimulus categories. Stimulus ratings did not differ between both groups. Brain data showed that BEGIN-smoking-stimuli led to enhanced mesolimbic responses (amygdala, hippocampus, insula) in dissonant compared to consonant smokers. In response to END-smoking-stimuli, dissonant smokers showed reduced mesocortical responses (orbitofrontal cortex, subcallosal cortex) compared to consonant smokers. These results suggest that smoking stimuli with a high incentive value (BEGIN-smoking-stimuli) are more appetitive for dissonant than consonant smokers at least on the neural level. To the contrary, smoking stimuli with low incentive value (END-smoking-stimuli) seem to be less appetitive for dissonant smokers than consonant smokers. These differences might be one reason why dissonant smokers experience difficulties in translating their attitudes into an actual behavior change.
-
BACKGROUND AND PURPOSE: In PD, tissue damage occurs in specific cortical and subcortical regions. Conventional MR images have only limited capacity to depict these structural changes. The purpose of the current study was to investigate whether voxel-based MT imaging could indicate structural abnormalities beyond atrophy measurable with T1-weighted MR imaging. MATERIALS AND METHODS: Thirty-six patients with PD without dementia (9 in H&Y stage 1, thirteen in H&Y 2, eleven in H&Y 3, three in H&Y 4) and 23 age-matched control subjects were studied with T1-weighted MR imaging and MT imaging. Voxel-based analyses of T1-weighted MR imaging was performed to investigate brain atrophy, while MT imaging was used to study abnormalities within existing tissue. Modulated GM and WM probability maps, sensitive to volume, and nonmodulated maps, indicative of tissue density, were obtained from T1-weighted MR imaging. Effects seen on MTR images, but absent on density maps, were attributed to damage of existing tissue. RESULTS: Contrary to T1-weighted MR imaging, MT imaging was sensitive to the progression of brain pathology of the neocortex and paraventricular WM. MTR images and T1-based volume images, but not density images, showed a progression of disease in the olfactory cortex, indicating the occurrence of atrophy as well as damage to existing tissue in this region. MTR images revealed bilateral damage to the SN, while T1-weighted MR imaging only showed left-sided abnormalities. CONCLUSIONS: The findings suggest that voxel-based MT imaging permits a whole-brain unbiased investigation of CNS structural integrity in PD and may be a valuable tool for identifying structural damage occurring without or before measurable atrophy.
-
INTRODUCTION: Studies investigating sexual arousal exist, yet there are diverging findings on the underlying neural mechanisms with regard to sexual orientation. Moreover, sexual arousal effects have often been confounded with general arousal effects. Hence, it is still unclear which structures underlie the sexual arousal response in homosexual and heterosexual men. AIM: Neural activity and subjective responses were investigated in order to disentangle sexual from general arousal. Considering sexual orientation, differential and conjoint neural activations were of interest. METHODS: The functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) study focused on the neural networks involved in the processing of sexual stimuli in 21 male participants (11 homosexual, 10 heterosexual). Both groups viewed pictures with erotic content as well as aversive and neutral stimuli. The erotic pictures were subdivided into three categories (most sexually arousing, least sexually arousing, and rest) based on the individual subjective ratings of each participant. MAIN OUTCOME MEASURES: Blood oxygen level-dependent responses measured by fMRI and subjective ratings. RESULTS: A conjunction analysis revealed conjoint neural activation related to sexual arousal in thalamus, hypothalamus, occipital cortex, and nucleus accumbens. Increased insula, amygdala, and anterior cingulate gyrus activation could be linked to general arousal. Group differences emerged neither when viewing the most sexually arousing pictures compared with highly arousing aversive pictures nor compared with neutral pictures. CONCLUSION: Results suggest that a widespread neural network is activated by highly sexually arousing visual stimuli. A partly distinct network of structures underlies sexual and general arousal effects. The processing of preferred, highly sexually arousing stimuli recruited similar structures in homosexual and heterosexual males.
-
Physiological responses in the Concealed Information Test (CIT) are known to depend on the depth of encoding critical items; CIT questions commonly refer to knowledge about critical items. It is unclear to what extent (1) different modes of item handling in a mock-crime, and (2) alternative questioning formats, e.g. asking about participants' particular actions with the critical items, influence the physiological responses. In the presented mock-crime study with fifty-three participants, two questioning formats, i.e. "Did you see …?" (viewing questioning) and "Did you steal …?" (stealing questioning), were compared between subjects. The mode of encoding, stealing vs. merely viewing the critical objects, was varied within subject. Skin conductance, electrocardiogram, respiration, and finger pulse were recorded. For both questioning formats and each physiological measure, physiological responses to stolen as well as merely viewed objects differed from those to irrelevant objects. Considering viewing questioning, responses to stolen and merely viewed objects did not differ, with the exception of greater phasic decreases of heart rate for stolen objects. Considering stealing questioning, responses to stolen objects exceeded those to merely viewed objects with each physiological measure. The statistically proven interaction between mode of encoding a particular object and questioning format sheds light on the factors influencing the physiological responses in a CIT. The level of subjective significance of a particular item might emerge interactively from the mode of item handling and the questioning format.
-
In an fMRI study, effects of contingency awareness on conditioned responses were assessed in three groups comprising 118 subjects. A differential fear-conditioning paradigm with visual conditioned stimuli, an electrical unconditioned stimulus and two distractors was applied. The instructed aware group was informed about the contingencies, whereas the distractors prevented contingency detection in the unaware group. The third group (learned aware) was not informed about the contingencies, but learned them despite the distractors. Main effects of contingency awareness on conditioned responses emerged in several brain structures. Post hoc tests revealed differential dorsal anterior cingulate, insula and ventral striatum responses in aware conditioning only, whereas the amygdala was activated independent of contingency awareness. Differential responses of the hippocampus were specifically observed in learned aware subjects, indicating a role in the development of contingency awareness. The orbitofrontal cortex showed varying response patterns: lateral structures showed higher responses in instructed aware than unaware subjects, the opposite was true for medial parts. Conditioned subjective and electrodermal responses emerged only in the two aware groups. These results confirm the independence of conditioned amygdala responses from contingency awareness and indicate specific neural circuits for different aspects of fear acquisition in unaware, learned aware and instructed aware subjects.
-
During observation of an ambiguous Necker cube, our percept changes spontaneously although the external stimulus does not. An EEG paradigm allowing time-resolved EEG measurement during endogenous perceptual reversals recently revealed a chain of ERP correlates beginning with an early occipital positivity at around 130 ms (Reversal Positivity, "RP"). In order to better understand the functional role of this RP, we investigated its relation to the P100, which is spatiotemporally close, typically occurring 100 ms after onset of a visual stimulus at occipital electrodes. We compared the relation of the ERP amplitudes to varying sizes of ambiguous Necker cubes. The main results are: (1) The P100 amplitude increases monotonically with stimulus size but is independent of the participants' percept. (2) The RP, in contrast, is percept-related and largely unaffected by stimulus size. (3) A similar pattern to RP was found for reaction times: They depend on the percept but not on stimulus size. We speculate that the P100 reflects processing of elementary visual features, while the RP is related to a processing conflict during 3D interpretation that precedes a reversal. The present results indicate that low-level visual processing (related to stimulus size) and (relative) high-level processing (related to perceptual reversal) occur in close spatial and temporal vicinity.
Erkunden
Eintragsart
- Zeitschriftenartikel (229)
Thema
- Humans
- 5-HTTLPR (1)
- absorption (1)
- Abstinence (1)
- Acoustic Stimulation (5)
- Acoustic Stimulation/methods (6)
- action mapping (1)
- action observation (1)
- Activities of Daily Living (2)
- Adaptation, Physiological (1)
- Adaptation, Physiological/physiology (2)
- Addictive disorders (1)
- Adolescent (24)
- Adult (173)
- *Affect (1)
- Affect (1)
- Affect/drug effects (1)
- Affect/*physiology (1)
- Affective Symptoms (1)
- Affective Symptoms/*physiopathology/psychology (1)
- Age Distribution (1)
- Age Factors (3)
- Aged (16)
- Aged, 80 and over (8)
- aging (1)
- Aging (1)
- Aging/*physiology (1)
- Aging/*physiology/*psychology (1)
- Aging/*psychology (1)
- Aging/psychology (1)
- *Albinism (1)
- Alcohol Drinking/*psychology (1)
- *Alcoholic Beverages (1)
- Algorithms (3)
- Alleles (3)
- alpha-Amylases/metabolism (1)
- Alpha Rhythm (1)
- Altered states of consciousness (3)
- altered states of consciousness (1)
- Alzheimer Disease (1)
- Alzheimer's disease (1)
- Ambiguous figures (1)
- Ambiguous figures, Necker cube, Multistable perception, EEG, ERPs, Bayesian inference (1)
- Amphetamine-Related Disorders/*diagnosis/*physiopathology (1)
- Amphetamine-Related Disorders/physiopathology/psychology/rehabilitation (1)
- amygdala (2)
- Amygdala (3)
- Amygdala/anatomy & histology/blood supply/physiology (1)
- Amygdala/blood supply (1)
- Amygdala/blood supply/metabolism (1)
- Amygdala/blood supply/physiology (1)
- Amygdala/*blood supply/*physiology (1)
- Amygdala/blood supply/*physiopathology (1)
- Amygdala/*metabolism (1)
- Amygdala/pathology (1)
- Amygdala/*physiology (1)
- Amygdala/physiology (7)
- Amygdala/physiopathology (2)
- Amygdala/*physiopathology (1)
- Analysis of Variance (17)
- Anger (1)
- Animals (8)
- Anterior cingulate (1)
- *Anthroposophy (1)
- Anthroposophy (1)
- Anticipation, Psychological/physiology (1)
- *Antipsychotic Agents (1)
- Antipsychotic Agents (1)
- Antipsychotic Agents/therapeutic use (1)
- Anxiety (1)
- Anxiety/diagnosis/etiology (1)
- Anxiety Disorders/physiopathology/*psychology (1)
- Anxiety/epidemiology (1)
- Anxiety/psychology (2)
- Anxiety/*psychology (1)
- *Anxiety/psychology (1)
- Aphasia/*physiopathology (1)
- Aphasia/physiopathology/psychology (1)
- Appetitive Behavior/*physiology (1)
- *Arousal (1)
- Arousal (11)
- Arousal/*drug effects (1)
- Arousal/physiology (5)
- Arousal/*physiology (2)
- Arrhythmias, Cardiac (2)
- Artifacts (2)
- ASD (1)
- Asperger Syndrome/diagnosis/*psychology (1)
- Asperger Syndrome/*physiopathology (1)
- Association (1)
- Association Learning (2)
- Association Learning/*physiology (2)
- Association Learning/physiology (1)
- Atmosphere (2)
- Atrophy/etiology (1)
- *Attention (2)
- Attention (5)
- Attention Deficit Disorder with Hyperactivity (1)
- Attention/drug effects/*physiology (1)
- Attention/*physiology (8)
- Attention/physiology (9)
- *Attitude (1)
- Attitude (1)
- Attitude of Health Personnel (2)
- Attitude to Death (1)
- Auditory Pathways/anatomy & histology/*growth & development/physiology (1)
- Auditory Perception/*physiology (7)
- Auditory Perception/physiology (1)
- *Auditory Perception/physiology (1)
- Auditory Threshold/*physiology (1)
- *Autism Spectrum Disorder (1)
- Autism spectrum disorder (1)
- *Autism Spectrum Disorder/diagnosis (1)
- *Autistic Disorder (1)
- Autogenic Training (1)
- *Automobile Driving (2)
- *Automobiles (1)
- Autonomic measures (1)
- Autonomic Nervous System (3)
- Autonomic Nervous System/*physiology (1)
- Aversive Therapy (1)
- Avoidance Learning/physiology (1)
- Awakening (1)
- Awareness (3)
- *Awareness (1)
- Awareness/physiology (4)
- Awareness/*physiology (9)
- balanced time perspective (1)
- Ball Drawing Test (1)
- Basal Ganglia/blood supply/drug effects/*physiology (1)
- Basal Ganglia/blood supply/metabolism (1)
- Basal Ganglia/physiology (1)
- Bayes Theorem (1)
- Bed Rest (1)
- Behavior (1)
- Behavior, Addictive/*physiopathology (2)
- Behavior, Addictive/*physiopathology/psychology (1)
- Behavior/physiology (2)
- Behavior Therapy (1)
- Beta oscillation (1)
- *Beta Rhythm (1)
- Beta Rhythm (1)
- *Big Data (1)
- Big data (1)
- Biofeedback, Psychology (3)
- Biological Clocks (1)
- Biological Clocks/physiology (1)
- Biomechanical Phenomena (1)
- bistability (1)
- Bistable perception (1)
- Blinking (1)
- Blinking/*physiology (2)
- *Blood (1)
- Blood Pressure (3)
- Blood Volume (1)
- Blood Volume/physiology (1)
- Body boundaries (1)
- *Boredom (3)
- Boredom (2)
- boredom proneness (1)
- Brain (7)
- Brain/anatomy & histology/blood supply/*physiology (1)
- Brain/anatomy & histology/*physiology (2)
- Brain/blood supply/*drug effects/physiology (1)
- Brain/*blood supply/metabolism (1)
- Brain/blood supply/pathology (2)
- Brain/blood supply/*physiology (1)
- Brain/*blood supply/*physiology (2)
- Brain/*blood supply/physiology (2)
- Brain/*diagnostic imaging/*physiology (1)
- Brain Diseases (1)
- Brain/*drug effects/physiology (1)
- Brain Injuries/pathology/*physiopathology (1)
- Brain Injury, Chronic/physiopathology/*psychology (1)
- Brain Mapping (29)
- *Brain Mapping (11)
- Brain Mapping/methods (5)
- Brain Mapping/methods/*psychology (1)
- Brain/*pathology (1)
- Brain/pathology/physiology (1)
- Brain/*physiology (11)
- Brain/physiology (7)
- *Brain/physiology (1)
- Brain/*physiopathology (4)
- Brain/physiopathology (4)
- Brain Stem/physiology (1)
- Brain Waves/*physiology (2)
- Bromocriptine/*pharmacology (1)
- *Buddhism (1)
- Bulimia Nervosa/*pathology (1)
- Bulimia Nervosa/*physiopathology/*psychology (1)
- Bulimia/*pathology (1)
- Carbamide Peroxide (1)
- cardiovascular (1)
- Case-Control Studies (5)
- Catechol O-Methyltransferase/genetics (1)
- Catechol O-Methyltransferase/*genetics (1)
- Central Nervous System Depressants/pharmacology (1)
- Central Nervous System/*physiology (1)
- Centrifugation (1)
- Cerebellum/physiology (1)
- Cerebellum/*physiology (1)
- Cerebral Cortex (4)
- Cerebral Cortex/blood supply/physiology (1)
- Cerebral Cortex/*blood supply/physiology (1)
- Cerebral Cortex/*pathology (1)
- Cerebral Cortex/pathology/*physiopathology (1)
- Cerebral Cortex/physiology (4)
- Cerebral Cortex/*physiology (10)
- Cerebral Cortex/*physiopathology (5)
- Cerebral Cortex/physiopathology (5)
- Cerebral Infarction/physiopathology/psychology (1)
- *Cerebrovascular Circulation (1)
- Cerebrovascular Circulation (3)
- Cerebrovascular Circulation/*physiology (2)
- Cerebrovascular Circulation/physiology (2)
- Child (4)
- Child, Preschool (3)
- Choice Behavior (1)
- Choice Behavior/*physiology (2)
- Chronic Disease (1)
- Chronobiology Phenomena (1)
- *Circadian Rhythm/drug effects (1)
- Circadian Rhythm/physiology (1)
- classical conditioning (1)
- Cocaine dependence (1)
- Cocaine-Related Disorders/*physiopathology (1)
- *Cognition (5)
- Cognition (4)
- Cognition Disorders/*etiology (1)
- Cognition Disorders/etiology/*physiopathology (1)
- Cognition/*drug effects/physiology (1)
- Cognition/*physiology (5)
- Cognition/physiology (1)
- *Cognitive Behavioral Therapy (2)
- Cognitive Behavioral Therapy/*methods (1)
- Cognitive Dysfunction/etiology/*physiopathology (1)
- cognitive reappraisal (1)
- Cognitive time scales (1)
- Cohort Studies (1)
- Coitus/*physiology (1)
- color effects (1)
- Communicable Disease Control (1)
- *Communication (1)
- Communication (1)
- Comprehension/*physiology (1)
- *Comprehension/physiology (1)
- computational modeling (1)
- Computer Simulation (2)
- *Computer Simulation (1)
- concealed information test (1)
- Concealed information test (1)
- Concept Formation (1)
- Conditioning, Classical (7)
- Conditioning, Classical/drug effects/*physiology (1)
- Conditioning, Classical/*physiology (7)
- Conditioning, Classical/physiology (1)
- Conditioning, Operant/physiology (1)
- Conditioning, Operant/*physiology (2)
- Conditioning, Psychological (2)
- Conditioning, Psychological/*drug effects (1)
- Conditioning, Psychological/*drug effects/physiology (1)
- Conditioning, Psychological/physiology (3)
- Conditioning, Psychological/*physiology (3)
- Conflict, Psychological (1)
- Consciousness (2)
- *Consciousness Disorders (1)
- Consciousness Disorders/chemically induced (1)
- Consciousness/*drug effects (1)
- Consciousness/*physiology (5)
- *Consciousness/physiology (1)
- Contingent negative variation (1)
- *Contingent Negative Variation (1)
- contingent negative variation (CNV) (1)
- Contingent Negative Variation/*physiology (5)
- Contingent Negative Variation/physiology (1)
- Contraception Behavior/psychology (1)
- Contraceptives, Oral (1)
- Contraceptives, Oral/*pharmacology/therapeutic use (1)
- Corpus Striatum/physiology (1)
- Corpus Striatum/*physiopathology (1)
- *Cortical Synchronization (1)
- Cortisol (1)
- *COVID-19 (1)
- *COVID-19/epidemiology (1)
- *Crime (1)
- Crime/psychology (2)
- criticality (1)
- Cross-Over Studies (1)
- Cross-Sectional Studies (2)
- Cued recall (1)
- *Cues (3)
- Cues (5)
- Current source density (1)
- Dancing (1)
- Dancing/physiology (1)
- *Data Display (1)
- Data Interpretation, Statistical (6)
- Databases, Factual (1)
- Databases, Factual/statistics & numerical data (1)
- *Deception (4)
- Decision (1)
- *Decision Making (1)
- Decision Making (2)
- Decision Making/physiology (1)
- Decision Making/*physiology (1)
- Decompression (1)
- Delay Discounting/*physiology (1)
- Delta Rhythm (1)
- Dementia (1)
- Depersonalization/chemically induced (1)
- Depersonalization/*complications (1)
- Depression/diagnosis/etiology (1)
- Depth Perception/*physiology (3)
- Desensitization, Immunologic (1)
- *Desensitization, Psychologic (1)
- Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (1)
- Diet (1)
- Digestive System Physiological Phenomena (1)
- Discrimination Learning (1)
- Discrimination Learning/physiology (1)
- Discrimination, Psychological (2)
- Discrimination, Psychological/*physiology (3)
- *Discrimination, Psychological/physiology (1)
- disembodiment (1)
- Distant healing (1)
- Distant intention (1)
- Distributed-practice effect (1)
- DNA Mutational Analysis (1)
- DNA Primers (1)
- Documentation (1)
- Dominance, Cerebral/physiology (3)
- Dopamine Agonists/adverse effects/*pharmacology (1)
- Dopamine Agonists/*pharmacology (1)
- Dose-Response Relationship, Drug (2)
- Double-Blind Method (6)
- Dreams/physiology (1)
- Drug Combinations (1)
- Duration discrimination (1)
- duration reproduction (1)
- *Echo-Planar Imaging (1)
- Echo-Planar Imaging (2)
- Education (1)
- EEG (2)
- *Ego (1)
- Electric Conductivity (2)
- Electric Injuries (1)
- Electric Stimulation (7)
- Electric Stimulation Therapy (1)
- Electrocardiography (6)
- Electrocardiography/methods (1)
- Electrodermal activity (1)
- *Electroencephalography (5)
- Electroencephalography (20)
- Electroencephalography/instrumentation/*methods (1)
- Electroencephalography/methods (3)
- Electroencephalography/*methods (1)
- Electroencephalography/*statistics & numerical data (1)
- Electromagnetic Fields (4)
- Electromagnetic Phenomena (1)
- Electromyography (2)
- Electrophysiology (1)
- Electroretinography/methods (1)
- Electroshock (1)
- Embodiment (1)
- Emotion (1)
- emotion regulation (2)
- *Emotional Regulation/physiology (1)
- Emotions (8)
- *Emotions (5)
- Emotions/*physiology (19)
- Emotions/physiology (6)
- End-of-life care (1)
- Environment (1)
- Epilepsy (3)
- Epistasis, Genetic (1)
- Ergonomics (1)
- Erotica (2)
- *Erotica (1)
- *Erotica/psychology (1)
- *Essential Tremor (1)
- essential tremor (1)
- Essential Tremor/complications/*physiopathology (1)
- Ethanol/*pharmacology (1)
- Ethanol/pharmacology (1)
- Ethnicity (1)
- Event-related desynchronization (1)
- Event-related potential (1)
- Event-related potentials (1)
- Event-Related Potentials, P300/*physiology (2)
- Event-Related Potentials, P300/physiology (1)
- *Evoked Potentials (1)
- Evoked Potentials (2)
- Evoked Potentials/*physiology (4)
- Evoked Potentials/physiology (7)
- *Evoked Potentials, Somatosensory (1)
- *Evoked Potentials, Visual (2)
- Evoked Potentials, Visual (2)
- Evoked Potentials, Visual/*physiology (7)
- Evoked Potentials, Visual/physiology (3)
- executive dysfunction (1)
- Executive Function (1)
- Expressed Emotion (1)
- Expressed Emotion/*physiology (2)
- expressive suppression (1)
- Extinction, Psychological (4)
- Extinction, Psychological/*drug effects/physiology (1)
- Extinction, Psychological/*physiology (2)
- Extinction, Psychological/physiology (1)
- eyewitness (1)
- Face (1)
- Facial Expression (4)
- *Facial Expression (2)
- Factor Analysis, Statistical (3)
- false memory (1)
- False memory (2)
- Fatigue (1)
- *Fear (1)
- Fear (6)
- *Fear/drug effects (1)
- Fear/*drug effects (1)
- Fear/*drug effects/physiology (1)
- Fear learning (1)
- Fear/*physiology (11)
- Fear/physiology (1)
- Fear/*physiology/*psychology (1)
- Fear/psychology (2)
- Fear/*psychology (5)
- Feedback (1)
- Feedback/physiology (1)
- Feedback, Psychological (1)
- Feedback, Psychological/drug effects/physiology (1)
- Female (173)
- Fibromyalgia (1)
- Field Dependence-Independence (1)
- *Fingers (1)
- Fingers/blood supply (3)
- Fingers/physiology (2)
- First-person data (1)
- Fixation, Ocular (1)
- Fixation, Ocular/physiology (1)
- Floatation-REST (1)
- Fluid Shifts (2)
- Fluid Shifts/*physiology (1)
- FMI (1)
- fMRI (4)
- Follow-Up Studies (3)
- Food (1)
- Foot/physiology (2)
- forensic anthropology (1)
- forensic science (1)
- Forest therapy (1)
- Forests (1)
- Forgetting (1)
- *Form Perception (1)
- Form Perception (2)
- Form Perception/*physiology (2)
- Fourier Analysis (1)
- *Free will (1)
- Frontal Lobe (2)
- Frontal Lobe/blood supply (1)
- Frontal Lobe/blood supply/*physiology (1)
- Frontal Lobe/*physiology (2)
- Frontal Lobe/physiology (5)
- Frontal Lobe/physiopathology (2)
- Functional Laterality (2)
- Functional Laterality/physiology (8)
- Functional Laterality/*physiology (3)
- Functional magnetic resonance imaging (1)
- Functional Neuroimaging (3)
- Galvanic Skin Response (10)
- Galvanic Skin Response/genetics (1)
- Galvanic Skin Response/physiology (11)
- Galvanic Skin Response/*physiology (5)
- Ganzfeld (1)
- *Gender Identity (1)
- *Gene-Environment Interaction (1)
- Gene Frequency (1)
- Genetic Predisposition to Disease (1)
- Genotype (5)
- Germany (1)
- Germany/epidemiology (1)
- Germany, West (1)
- Goals (1)
- Gonadal Steroid Hormones/*blood (1)
- Gonadal Steroid Hormones/*pharmacology (1)
- *Gravitation (1)
- Gravitation (1)
- Gravity Sensing (1)
- Guilt (1)
- Gymnastics/*physiology (1)
- Gyrus Cinguli/blood supply/*physiology (1)
- Gyrus Cinguli/physiology (3)
- Gyrus Cinguli/physiopathology (1)
- Habituation, Psychophysiologic (3)
- Hallucinations/genetics/*physiopathology (1)
- Hallucinogens/*administration & dosage (1)
- Hallucinogens/adverse effects/*pharmacology (1)
- Hand/physiology (2)
- Hand Strength/*physiology (1)
- Happiness (1)
- Headache (2)
- Health Personnel/*psychology (1)
- Hearing Disorders (1)
- Heart (2)
- Heart/*physiology (1)
- Heart Rate (12)
- heart rate (1)
- Heart Rate/physiology (6)
- Heart Rate/*physiology (1)
- *Heart Rate/physiology (1)
- heartbeat-counting task (1)
- heartbeat-evoked potential (HEP) (1)
- Hematologic Neoplasms/diagnosis/*psychology (1)
- Hemodynamics (5)
- Hemodynamics/physiology (4)
- Hemodynamics/*physiology (5)
- Heterosexuality/*physiology/psychology (1)
- Hippocampus/blood supply/metabolism (1)
- Hippocampus/physiology (1)
- Hippocampus/*physiology (1)
- Hippocampus/physiopathology (1)
- Homeostasis (1)
- *Homosexuality, Male/psychology (1)
- Hospitalization (1)
- Hospitals (1)
- Hospitals, General (1)
- Hydrocortisone/analysis/*metabolism (1)
- Hydrocortisone/metabolism (2)
- Hydrocortisone/*pharmacology (2)
- Hydrocortisone/pharmacology (2)
- Hydrocortisone/*physiology (1)
- Hypertension (3)
- Hypothalamus/*metabolism (1)
- Illusions/*physiology (1)
- Image Enhancement (1)
- Image Interpretation, Computer-Assisted (2)
- Image Processing, Computer-Assisted (19)
- *Image Processing, Computer-Assisted (2)
- Image Processing, Computer-Assisted/methods (4)
- *Imagery, Psychotherapy (2)
- *Imagery, Psychotherapy/methods (1)
- *Imagination (1)
- Imagination/*physiology (2)
- *Imagination/physiology (1)
- imaging genetics (1)
- Imaging, Three-Dimensional (1)
- Impulsive Behavior/*pathology/*physiopathology (1)
- Impulsive Behavior/*physiology (1)
- impulsivity (1)
- Impulsivity (1)
- *INDEL Mutation (1)
- Individuality (1)
- *Individuality (3)
- Industry (1)
- Inhibition, Psychological (2)
- *Inhibition, Psychological (1)
- Injections/*psychology (1)
- Intelligence/*genetics (1)
- Intention (3)
- *Intention (3)
- Intention to act (1)
- Interoception (1)
- *Interoception/physiology (1)
- Interoception/*physiology (1)
- interoceptive awareness (1)
- Interval of subjective uniformity (1)
- Japan (1)
- Jet Lag Syndrome/diagnosis/*physiopathology (1)
- *Judgment (1)
- Judgment (2)
- Judgment/*drug effects/physiology (1)
- Judgment/*physiology (2)
- Judgment/physiology (2)
- Karnofsky Performance Status (1)
- *Knowledge of Results, Psychological (1)
- Lag effect (1)
- Language (2)
- *Language (2)
- Learning (5)
- *Learning (2)
- Learning/*drug effects/physiology (1)
- Learning/physiology (1)
- Learning/*physiology (5)
- Libet experiment (2)
- *Libet task (1)
- *Libido (1)
- Lie Detection/*psychology (3)
- Life Change Events (2)
- Lightning (2)
- Lightning Injuries (1)
- Likelihood Functions (2)
- Limbic System/*physiopathology (1)
- Linear Models (3)
- Logistic Models (3)
- long-range temporal correlations (1)
- Low Back Pain (1)
- Lower Body Negative Pressure (3)
- Lucidity (1)
- Magnetic Resonance Imaging (50)
- *Magnetic Resonance Imaging (7)
- Magnetic Resonance Imaging/methods (10)
- Magnetic Resonance Imaging/*methods (3)
- Magnetic Resonance Imaging/methods/psychology (1)
- Magnetic Resonance Imaging/*statistics & numerical data (1)
- Magnetics (1)
- Magnetoencephalography (1)
- Male (164)
- Matched-Pair Analysis (1)
- Mathematics (2)
- Meaning in life (1)
- Mechanoreceptors/physiology (1)
- *Meditation (5)
- Meditation (4)
- meditation (1)
- Meditation/*methods (1)
- Meditation/methods/*psychology (1)
- Meditation/*psychology (1)
- memory (1)
- Memory (1)
- *Memory (1)
- *Memory Consolidation (1)
- Memory Consolidation/physiology (1)
- Memory/drug effects (1)
- Memory, Long-Term/physiology (1)
- Memory/*physiology (3)
- Memory/physiology (2)
- *Memory, Short-Term (1)
- Memory, Short-Term (1)
- Memory, Short-Term/*physiology (4)
- Mental Disorders (1)
- Mental Disorders/*epidemiology/*psychology (1)
- Mental Healing/*psychology (1)
- Mental Processes/drug effects/physiology (1)
- Mental Processes/*physiology (2)
- Mental Processes/physiology (1)
- Mental Recall (1)
- Mental Recall/*physiology (1)
- Mental Recall/physiology (1)
- metacognition (1)
- Methamphetamine (2)
- Methamphetamine dependence (1)
- Middle Aged (46)
- Migraine Disorders (3)
- military crisis (1)
- *Mindfulness (2)
- Mindfulness (1)
- mindfulness (1)
- Misinformation paradigm (2)
- Models, Anatomic (1)
- Models, Biological (1)
- Models, Genetic (1)
- Models, Neurological (1)
- *Models, Neurological (1)
- *Models, Psychological (1)
- Models, Psychological (1)
- Models, Statistical (1)
- *Models, Theoretical (1)
- Mood Disorders (1)
- *Motion Perception (1)
- Motion Perception/*physiology (3)
- *Motion Sickness/drug therapy (1)
- Motivation (3)
- *Motivation (2)
- Motivation/*physiology (1)
- Motor Activity (1)
- Motor Activity/*physiology (2)
- Motor Cortex/*physiology (4)
- Motor Cortex/physiology (1)
- motor imagery (1)
- motor simulation (1)
- *Movement (2)
- Movement (2)
- Movement/*physiology (4)
- Movement/physiology (1)
- Multiple Sclerosis/complications/*physiopathology (1)
- Multiple Sclerosis, Relapsing-Remitting/*complications (1)
- Multivariate Analysis (2)
- Muscle Relaxation (1)
- *Museums (1)
- Music/*psychology (1)
- Music therapy (1)
- Myocardial Infarction (2)
- Nature therapy (1)
- Nausea/etiology (1)
- Necker cube (2)
- Necker-Zeno model (1)
- Neoplasms (1)
- Neostriatum/physiology (1)
- Neostriatum/*physiopathology (1)
- Nerve Net/anatomy & histology/physiology (1)
- Nerve Net/*growth & development/physiology (1)
- Nerve Net/*physiology (2)
- Nerve Net/physiology (1)
- Neural Inhibition (2)
- Neural Inhibition/*physiology (1)
- Neural Pathways/anatomy & histology/*physiology (1)
- Neural Pathways/metabolism (1)
- Neural Pathways/*physiology (1)
- Neural Pathways/physiopathology (1)
- neurocognition (1)
- Neuroimaging (1)
- Neuromuscular Diseases (1)
- Neuronal Plasticity/physiology (1)
- Neurons/*physiology (2)
- Neurophenomenology (1)
- Neuropsychological Tests (12)
- Neuropsychological Tests/statistics & numerical data (1)
- Neurotic Disorders (1)
- Nicotine/pharmacology (1)
- Nucleus accumbens (1)
- Nucleus Accumbens (1)
- Nucleus Accumbens/physiology (1)
- Nurses (1)
- *Nystagmus, Pathologic (1)
- Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder/*diagnosis/*physiopathology (1)
- Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder/*physiopathology/psychology (1)
- Occipital Lobe (2)
- Occipital Lobe/blood supply/metabolism (1)
- Occipital Lobe/physiology (3)
- Occipital Lobe/*physiology (1)
- OCT (1)
- Odorants (2)
- Oneness beliefs (1)
- Optic Chiasm (1)
- *Optic Nerve (1)
- Optical coherence tomography (1)
- Optical Illusions (2)
- *Optical Illusions (1)
- Optical Illusions/physiology (1)
- Optical Illusions/*physiology (4)
- Oral contraceptives (1)
- Organ Size (2)
- Orgasm (1)
- Orgasm/*physiology (1)
- Orientation/physiology (1)
- Orientation/*physiology (1)
- Orientation, Spatial (1)
- orienting (1)
- Otolithic Membrane/physiology (1)
- Oxygen (1)
- Oxygen/blood (13)
- Pain (1)
- Pain Management/adverse effects/*methods/psychology (1)
- Pain Measurement (4)
- Pain/*physiopathology (2)
- *Paintings (1)
- Pandemics (1)
- *Pandemics (1)
- Paralysis (1)
- *Parapsychology (1)
- Parapsychology (3)
- Parietal Lobe/*physiology (3)
- Parkinson Disease (1)
- Parkinson Disease/*physiopathology/*therapy (1)
- passage of time (1)
- Pattern Recognition, Automated (1)
- Pattern Recognition, Physiological (1)
- *Pattern Recognition, Visual (2)
- Pattern Recognition, Visual (3)
- Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology (2)
- Pattern Recognition, Visual/*physiology (6)
- Perception (5)
- Perception/*physiology (3)
- Perceptual Closure/*physiology (1)
- perceptual deprivation (1)
- Perceptual Masking/*physiology (2)
- Periodicity (1)
- Peroxides/blood (1)
- Personal Satisfaction (1)
- Personality (5)
- *Personality (1)
- Personality Assessment (1)
- Personality Inventory (4)
- *Personality Tests (1)
- Perspectival contraction of time (1)
- Phenotype (3)
- Phobic Disorders/*pathology/psychology (1)
- Phobic Disorders/pathology/*psychology/*therapy (1)
- Phobic Disorders/*physiopathology (2)
- Phobic Disorders/*physiopathology/psychology (1)
- Phobic Disorders/physiopathology/psychology/*therapy (1)
- Phobic Disorders/*psychology (2)
- Photic Stimulation (36)
- *Photic Stimulation (5)
- Photic Stimulation/*instrumentation/methods (1)
- Photic Stimulation/methods (11)
- Photic Stimulation/*methods (1)
- Photoperiod (1)
- Physical Education and Training (1)
- Physical Stimulation (1)
- Physicians (1)
- *Placebo Effect (1)
- Placebos (2)
- Plethysmography (3)
- Plethysmography, Impedance (1)
- Plethysmography/methods (1)
- political views (1)
- Politics (1)
- *Polymorphism, Genetic (3)
- Polymorphism, Genetic/*genetics (1)
- Polymorphism, Single Nucleotide/genetics (1)
- Portugal (1)
- positive emotion (1)
- Positron-Emission Tomography (1)
- Postural Balance (1)
- Posture (5)
- Posture/*physiology (1)
- Practice, Psychological (1)
- Predictive Value of Tests (1)
- Prefrontal Cortex (1)
- Prefrontal Cortex/blood supply (1)
- Prefrontal Cortex/blood supply/physiopathology (1)
- Prefrontal Cortex/pathology (1)
- Prefrontal Cortex/*physiology (1)
- Prefrontal Cortex/physiology (2)
- Prefrontal Cortex/physiopathology (1)
- Prefrontal Cortex/*physiopathology (1)
- Pressoreceptors (1)
- Pressure (1)
- Prevalence (1)
- Problem Solving (1)
- Problem Solving/physiology (1)
- Problem Solving/*physiology (1)
- Prognosis (2)
- Promoter Regions, Genetic/genetics (1)
- Proportional Hazards Models (1)
- Proprioception (2)
- Prospective Studies (1)
- Psilocybin/*administration & dosage (1)
- Psilocybin/adverse effects/*pharmacology (1)
- Psychiatric Status Rating Scales (5)
- Psychoacoustics (1)
- Psycholinguistics (1)
- Psychological absorption (1)
- Psychological Theory (1)
- Psychometrics (2)
- Psychomotor Performance (3)
- Psychomotor Performance/*drug effects (1)
- Psychomotor Performance/*physiology (7)
- Psychomotor Performance/physiology (4)
- Psychophysics (5)
- Psychophysics/methods (1)
- Psychophysiologic Disorders (1)
- Psychophysiology (1)
- Psychophysiology/methods (1)
- Pulse (3)
- Qualitative Research (1)
- *Quality of Life (1)
- Quality of Life (2)
- Quantum cognition (1)
- Radiation (1)
- Random Allocation (1)
- Raynaud Disease (2)
- Reaction Time (14)
- Reaction time (1)
- *Reaction Time (1)
- Reaction Time/drug effects/genetics (1)
- Reaction Time/drug effects/*physiology (1)
- Reaction Time/*physiology (2)
- Reaction Time/physiology (15)
- *Readiness potential (1)
- Readiness potential (2)
- Receptor, Serotonin, 5-HT2A/*genetics (1)
- Receptors, Dopamine D2/*genetics (1)
- Recognition, Psychology/*physiology (2)
- Recognition, Psychology/physiology (1)
- Recovery (1)
- Recurrence (3)
- Reference Values (8)
- Reflex (1)
- Reflex, Startle (3)
- Reflex, Startle/physiology (1)
- Regional Blood Flow (1)
- Regional Blood Flow/physiology (3)
- Regression Analysis (4)
- *Reinforcement, Psychology (1)
- Reinforcement, Psychology (1)
- Relapse (2)
- Relaxation (2)
- Relaxation Therapy (2)
- *Repression, Psychology (2)
- Reproducibility of Results (8)
- Research (1)
- Research Design (1)
- Respiration (5)
- Respiratory Mechanics (1)
- Respiratory Mechanics/physiology (2)
- Respiratory Rate/physiology (2)
- Rest (1)
- Rest/physiology (1)
- *Retention, Psychology (1)
- Retention, Psychology (1)
- Retest Reliability (1)
- Retina (1)
- Retina/diagnostic imaging (2)
- *Retinal Diseases/complications (1)
- *Retinal Ganglion Cells/physiology (1)
- Retrospective Studies (4)
- Retrospective timing (1)
- Reversal Learning/physiology (1)
- Reverse Transcriptase Polymerase Chain Reaction (1)
- Reverse Transcriptase Polymerase Chain Reaction/methods (1)
- *Reward (4)
- Reward (4)
- Risk prediction (1)
- *Risk-Taking (1)
- ROC Curve (2)
- Safety (2)
- Saliva/chemistry (1)
- Saliva/metabolism (1)
- SARS-CoV-2 (1)
- Schizophrenia (1)
- Schizophrenia/drug therapy/*physiopathology (1)
- Schizophrenia/genetics/physiopathology (1)
- Schizophrenia, Paranoid/diagnostic imaging (1)
- Schizophrenia/*physiopathology (1)
- *Schizophrenic Psychology (1)
- Schizophrenic Psychology (2)
- schizotypal disorder (1)
- Schizotypal Personality Disorder/*complications/physiopathology (1)
- Seasons (1)
- self (1)
- Self (1)
- Self-Assessment (1)
- Self Concept (2)
- *Self Concept (2)
- self-control (1)
- Self-control (1)
- Self-Control/*psychology (1)
- Self-regulation (1)
- Self Report (3)
- Self-transcendence (1)
- sensation/perception (1)
- Sensation/*physiology (1)
- Sensitivity and Specificity (1)
- Sensory Receptor Cells (1)
- Sensory Thresholds/physiology (2)
- Serotonin 5-HT1 Receptor Agonists (1)
- Serotonin 5-HT2 Receptor Agonists (1)
- Serotonin/*metabolism (1)
- Serotonin Plasma Membrane Transport Proteins/*genetics (1)
- Serotonin Plasma Membrane Transport Proteins/*genetics/metabolism (1)
- Serotonin Plasma Membrane Transport Proteins/genetics/*physiology (1)
- Severity of Illness Index (1)
- *Sex Characteristics (4)
- Sex Characteristics (6)
- Sex differences (1)
- Sex Factors (4)
- *Sexual Behavior (1)
- Sexual Behavior/*physiology (4)
- Sexual Behavior/physiology (1)
- Sexual Behavior/*physiology/psychology (1)
- Sexual Behavior/*psychology (1)
- Sexual Desire (1)
- Sexual Motivation (1)
- Sexual Partners (1)
- Sexual responsiveness (1)
- Sick Role (3)
- Signal Processing, Computer-Assisted (3)
- Silence (1)
- Skin (2)
- Skin/blood supply (1)
- Skin/pathology (1)
- Skin Physiological Phenomena (3)
- Sleep (1)
- Sleep/*drug effects (1)
- Sleep/*physiology (1)
- Sleep/physiology (1)
- Slow cortical potential (2)
- *Slow cortical potential (1)
- Smell/*physiology (1)
- Smiling (2)
- Smoking/*physiopathology (1)
- Smoking/*physiopathology/psychology (2)
- Smoking/*psychology (1)
- *Social Behavior (1)
- Social Environment (2)
- socioeconomic crisis (1)
- *Socioeconomic Factors (1)
- Somatoform Disorders/complications (1)
- somatopsychic depersonalization (1)
- Somatosensory Cortex/physiology (1)
- Somatosensory Cortex/physiopathology (1)
- somatotopic mapping (1)
- Space Perception (1)
- Space Perception/drug effects (1)
- Space Perception/drug effects/*physiology (1)
- Space Perception/*physiology (3)
- Spacing effect (1)
- Spectrum Analysis (1)
- Speech Discrimination Tests (1)
- Speech Perception/*physiology (1)
- *Spiders (3)
- Spiders (3)
- Spirituality (1)
- *Sports (1)
- Starvation (1)
- Statistics as Topic (2)
- Statistics, Nonparametric (3)
- Stomach (1)
- Stress hormones (1)
- *Stress, Psychological (1)
- Stress, Psychological (2)
- Stress, Psychological/*genetics/metabolism (1)
- *Stress, Psychological/genetics/pathology/psychology (1)
- Stress, Psychological/*physiopathology (1)
- Stress, Psychological/*physiopathology/*psychology (1)
- Stress, Psychological/*psychology (1)
- Striatum (1)
- Students (1)
- Subjective shortening (1)
- Substance-Related Disorders (1)
- Substance-Related Disorders/*physiopathology (1)
- Substance-Related Disorders/physiopathology (1)
- Substance-Related Disorders/*physiopathology/*psychology (1)
- Suggestion (1)
- Supine Position/physiology (1)
- Surveys and Questionnaires (15)
- *Surveys and Questionnaires (2)
- *Surveys and Questionnaires/standards (1)
- Survival Analysis (1)
- Switzerland (1)
- Sympathetic Nervous System (1)
- Sympathetic Nervous System/physiology (1)
- Synaptic Transmission/physiology (1)
- Syncope, Vasovagal/physiopathology/psychology (1)
- *Task Performance and Analysis (1)
- Task Performance and Analysis (3)
- Telepathy (1)
- Temporal Lobe (2)
- temporal processing (1)
- Tension-Type Headache (2)
- Terminal Care (2)
- Terminally Ill (1)
- Test Anxiety Scale (1)
- Testing effect (1)
- Thalamus/blood supply/metabolism (1)
- Thalamus/physiology (2)
- Thalamus/physiopathology (2)
- *Theta Rhythm (2)
- Theta Rhythm (1)
- Thinking/physiology (2)
- Thorax (1)
- Thorax/blood supply (1)
- Tilt-Table Test (1)
- time awareness (1)
- Time Factors (20)
- time orientation (1)
- time perception (4)
- Time perception (6)
- *Time Perception (9)
- Time Perception/*drug effects (1)
- Time Perception/*drug effects/physiology (1)
- *Time Perception/physiology (3)
- Time Perception/*physiology (15)
- time perspective (3)
- Time processing (1)
- Timelessness (1)
- Tomography Scanners, X-Ray Computed (1)
- touch (1)
- Touch Perception/*physiology (2)
- Trait Sexual Motivation (1)
- Treatment Outcome (1)
- Tryptophan Hydroxylase/*genetics (1)
- TSST (1)
- Ukraine (1)
- Ultrasonography, Doppler, Transcranial (1)
- Uniformity illusion (1)
- Urea/analogs & derivatives/blood (1)
- *User-Computer Interface (1)
- Vagus Nerve (1)
- validation (1)
- Vascular Resistance (1)
- Vasoconstriction (1)
- Verbal Behavior (1)
- Verbal Behavior/*physiology (1)
- verbal transformation effect (1)
- Video Recording (2)
- Viscera (1)
- Visceral Afferents (1)
- vision (1)
- Vision, Binocular/*physiology (1)
- Vision, Binocular/physiology (1)
- Vision Disorders/etiology (1)
- Visual Acuity/*physiology (1)
- *Visual Cortex (1)
- Visual Cortex/*blood supply/*physiology (1)
- Visual Cortex/physiology (3)
- Visual Fields (2)
- Visual Fields/physiology (1)
- Visual Perception (7)
- *Visual Perception (1)
- Visual Perception/*physiology (14)
- Visual Perception/physiology (6)
- *Visual Perception/physiology (1)
- vmPFC (1)
- Vocabulary (2)
- Volition (2)
- Volition/physiology (1)
- Volition/*physiology (2)
- *Voluntary action (1)
- voxel-based morphometry (1)
- Waiting situation (1)
- Wakefulness (3)
- *Wakefulness/physiology (1)
- Weather (2)
- Weightlessness (1)
- *Wine (1)
- Work Schedule Tolerance (1)
- Work Schedule Tolerance/*physiology/*psychology (1)
- *Workload (1)
- Wounds and Injuries (1)
- Young Adult (62)
- ZTPI (1)